Perceptual-Motor Abilities of Infants in the 1 to 2 Month Period: Difference between revisions

m (deleted sentence on intro)
m (A paragraph was repetead)
 
(35 intermediate revisions by 5 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
<div class="editorbox"> '''Original Editor '''- [[User:Robin Tacchetti|Robin Tacchetti]] based on the course by [https://members.physio-pedia.com/course_tutor/pam-versfeld/ Pam Versfeld]<br>
<div class="editorbox"> '''Original Editor '''- [https://members.physio-pedia.com/course_tutor/pam-versfeld/ Pam Versfeld]  
'''Top Contributors''' - {{Special:Contributors/{{FULLPAGENAME}}}}</div>
'''Top Contributors''' - {{Special:Contributors/{{FULLPAGENAME}}}}</div>


== Introduction ==
== Infant development: 1-2 month period ==
During the 1 to 2 month old period infants gain various new skills using their perceptual-motor abilities. Perception involves understanding, categorising and translating sensory information.  Examples of perceptual ability include:


* Recognising:
** Familiar faces vs. non-familiar faces
** The voice of a family member
* Distinguishing:
** The height of an object
** The colour of an object
** The depth of an object


Motor ability refers to an infant's capacity to control their body movements. Motor abilities encompass all movements of the body including, waving, kicking, reaching, grasping, etc. Perceptual and motor abilities are collaborative as infants use their perceptual skills to decide on the correct motor action to undertake.<ref>California Department of Education. Perceptual and Motor Development Domain. Last reviewed 2021. Accessible at:https://www.cde.ca.gov/sp/cd/re/itf09percmotdev.asp#percdev</ref> 
This is an exciting period as it the time when infants’ perceptual-motor behaviour shifts from spontaneous movements to movements that become increasingly intentional as the infant uses longer periods of being awake and alert to explore and connect with their social and physical environment.  


Initially, an infant's perceptual-motor behaviour is spontaneous. During the 1 to 2 month period, infants transition from spontaneous movement to intentional movements. During this period of development, you can expect to see the following:<ref name=":1">Versfeld P. Perceptual-Motor Abilities in the Infant 1-2 Month Period Course. Physioplus. 2022.</ref>
* When awake and alert infants alternate between period of activity and periods of quiet
* Movements shift from writhing to fidgety
* Infants gain greater control of their head posture and movements, increasing their ability for visual reach and information gathering
* Greater head control and movements allowing increased ability to visually reach and gather information
* They look towards interesting sounds and visual events in the environment
* Turning towards sounds and visual events within their environment
* They actively explore their bodies, clothing and surrounding surfaces with their hands and their feet
* Exploring their bodies, clothing and surrounding surfaces with their hands and feet
* They are becoming increasingly self-aware and develop an awareness of their own bodies relative to the surrounding physical environment
* Successful and intentional reaching
* Reaching becomes more intentional and successful.


== Communication and Social Interaction ==
=== Communication and Social Behaviour ===
[[File:PV3.jpg|thumb|alt=|278x278px|Infant smiling]]
During this period infants’ communication and social behaviour increases and become more complex, sustained and expressive.<ref>Farran LK, Yoo H, Lee CC, Bowman DD, Oller DK. [https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6856762/pdf/fpsyg-10-02374.pdf Temporal Coordination in Mother-Infant Vocal Interaction: A Cross-Cultural Comparison.] Front Psychol. 2019 Nov 8;10:2374.</ref>
Newborns in the 1-2 month period will begin to demonstrate more complex, sustained and expressive social behaviours. Some of the types of communication and social development you might notice in an infant in the 1 to 2 month period are listed below:<ref name=":1" />  
* First smiles
* Turns head towards voices and other sounds within the environment
* Quietens their limbs movements
* Smiles in response to sounds


* Interest in faces
* First smiles occur
* Recognises familiar faces
* They turn their heads towards voices and other sounds in the environment
* Makes eye contact
* Quieten their limb movements or smile in response to sounds
* Has different cries for different needs
* Show increasing interest in faces and recognise familiar faces
* Produces pre-speech sounds known as protophones, which includes grunts, coos, and gurgles
* Make eye contact
* Cry differently for different needs (e.g. hungry vs tired)
* Produce a range of pre-speech sounds known as protophones - that include grunts, coos and gurgles.


=== Caregiver Interaction ===
==== Vocalisation ====
[[File:PV5.jpg|left|alt=|225x225px|thumb|Caregiver-infant interaction]]
During this period infants produce a range of pre-speech sounds, known as protophones.<ref>Oller DK, Ramsay G, Bene E, Long HL, Griebel U. [https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8419580/pdf/rstb.2020.0255.pdf Protophones, the precursors to speech, dominate the human infant vocal landscape.] Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2021 Oct 25;376(1836):20200255. </ref>
Interactions between the caregiver and infant require clear cues from both the infant and the caregiver to facilitate responses to each other. Taking cues from one another allows the infant and caregiver to change or adapt their behaviour in response to the interaction. This mutual interactive environment teaches the infant communication and social interaction.<ref>Chung FF, Wan GH, Kuo SC, Lin KC, Liu HE. [https://bmcpregnancychildbirth.biomedcentral.com/articles/10.1186/s12884-018-1979-7 Mother–infant interaction quality and sense of parenting competence at six months postpartum for first-time mothers in Taiwan: a multiple time series design.] BMC pregnancy and childbirth. 2018 Dec;18(1):1-3.</ref>


* These sounds include grunts, squeals, coos, and vowel like sounds.
* Vocalisations becomes more extended and include changes in emotional tone (known as inflexion)
* Vocal behavior is often produced during social interaction but is also heard when infants are not attending to the environment.
This spontaneous vocalisation allows the infant to practice and explore different ways of producing a range of different sounds which provides the foundation for later babbling and words. <ref>Long HL, Bowman DD, Yoo H, Burkhardt-Reed MM, Bene ER, Oller DK. [https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7406057/pdf/pone.0224956.pdf Social and endogenous infant vocalizations.] PLoS One. 2020 Aug 5;15(8):e0224956.</ref>


==== Social interaction and mirroring ====
In the 1-2 month period infants continue to pay special attention to faces. They also start to recognise familiar faces and will smile at them.  Their ability to sustain social interaction increases and they are figuring out the process of turn taking which is an inherent aspect of a conversation. Adult social partners will often imitate infants’ facial expressions, lip and jaw movements and vocalisations which act as a mirror that allows the infant to “see” and “hear” copies of their own actions. 


==== Caregiver Speech ====
When speaking to infants, adults typically alter the acoustic properties of their speech in a variety of ways compared with how they speak to other adults; for example, they use higher pitch, increased pitch range that is making the voice go up and down in tone, more pitch variability, and slower speech rate. Recent research by Tanya Broesch and Gregory Bryant has shown that these vocal changes happen similarly, not only across industrialised populations but also in traditional societies.<ref>Broesch TL, Bryant GA. [https://www.researchgate.net/publication/271671965_Prosody_in_Infant-Directed_Speech_Is_Similar_Across_Western_and_Traditional_Cultures Prosody in infant-directed speech is similar across western and traditional cultures.] Journal of Cognition and Development. 2015 Jan 1;16(1):31-43.</ref>
Caregivers speak to infants in a language called motherese or baby talk. This type of infant directed speech allows adults to alter their acoustic properties to use a higher pitch, increased pitch range and variability, and a slower speech rate. Research shows that these vocal adjustments happen across all populations.<ref name=":1" />  


==== Infant Vocalisation ====
==== Spontaneous touches ====
To practise and explore sounds, infants will spontaneously vocalise, producing a range of sounds. Infants can alter these sounds by changing their emotional tone or inflexion. These different sounds will provide the foundation for later babbling and wordsPre-speech sounds such as grunts, squeals and coos are referred to as protophones.<ref name=":1" /> Full vowels, raspberries, yells, and whispers can be heard in infants when they are around 2 to 3 months old.<ref>Pretzer GM, Lopez LD, Walle EA, Warlaumont AS. [https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/S0163638318301401 Infant-adult vocal interaction dynamics depend on infant vocal type, child-directedness of adult speech, and timeframe]. Infant Behavior and Development. 2019 Nov 1;57:101325.</ref>
During this period infants continue to use their hands to gather information about the surfaces they encounter.  Contact with a surface is often associated with exploratory movements of the hand across the surface, or repeated flexion and extension of the fingers and graspingIndependent movements of the fingers are also frequently seen, especially when the infant is socially engaged or is paying attention to an object within reaching distance. <ref>DiMercurio A, Connell JP, Clark M, Corbetta D. [https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02613/full A naturalistic observation of spontaneous touches to the body and environment in the first 2 months of life.] Frontiers in psychology. 2018 Dec 18;9:2613.</ref>


*
=== Perceptual-Motor Development<ref>Baek S, Jaffe-Dax S, Emberson LL. How an infant's active response to structured experience supports perceptual-cognitive development. Prog Brain Res. 2020;254:167-186.</ref> ===


=== Mirroring ===
* During the 1-2 month period the infant is awake and alert for longer periods of time, increasingly responds to sounds and sights from the environment and gains more control of movements of the head and limbs
Caregivers will often "mirror" or imitate newborns' facial expressions, jaw and lip movements and vocalisations. This allows the infant to hear and see copies of their own activity.<ref name=":1" /> These parental responses are both instinctive and functionally important as they help the infant increase their social communication skills.<ref>Murray L, Bozicevic L, Ferrari PF, Vaillancourt K, Dalton L, Goodacre T, Chakrabarti B, Bicknell S, Cooper P, Stein A, De Pascalis L. [https://www.cde.ca.gov/sp/cd/re/itf09percmotdev.asp#percdev The effects of maternal mirroring on the development of infant social expressiveness: The case of infant cleft lip]. Neural plasticity. 2018 Dec 17;2018.</ref>
* Head control in supine is improving as the infant learns to hold the head steady in the midline as well as turn the head to locate interesting sights and sounds.
* When held upright infants can keep the head erect for extended periods of time.
* Neck rotation is still associated with extension and lateral flexion.
* Spontaneous movements of the lower extremities, along with the pull of gravity serve to stretch the hip and knee flexor muscles, leading to increased range of extension of the extremity joints.
* Postural control of the trunk in supine is improving but movement of the extremities still leads to lateral displacement and sometimes rotation of the pelvis.


<nowiki>**</nowiki> Infants begin to recognise familiar faces and smile at them during the 1 to 2 month period.
* Vigorous kicking of the lower extremities (LEs) is often associated with abduction of the shoulders and extension of the elbows as the infant works to maintain a steady torso.


Watch this video by Reach Out and Read to see how caregivers and infants communicate: 
* Kicking movements still show strong coupling between the hip and knee joints, however the ability to uncouple hip and knee emerges when the infant engages in goal directed actions of the foot, such as reaching to a target with the foot.
* Whole body general movements give way to fidgety general movements from around 9 weeks.


{{#ev:youtube|O8ETEajtfUs}}
* Infants start to reach successfully towards interesting objects within reach and start to use finger movements to explore objects.


== General Movements ==
* Infants actively explore surfaces with their feet and hands.
Within the 1 to 2 month period, infants transition from generalised writhing movements to fidgety movements. Writhing movements are characterised by complex, whole body movements including the arm, leg, trunk and neck in variable arrangements.<ref name=":0">Doroniewicz I, Ledwoń DJ, Affanasowicz A, Kieszczyńska K, Latos D, Matyja M, Mitas AW, Myśliwiec A. [https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7660095/#:~:text=A%20large%20percentage%20of%20newborns,analyzed%20group%20of%20healthy%20newborns. Writhing movement detection in newborns on the second and third day of life using pose-based feature machine learning classification]. Sensors. 2020 Jan;20(21):5986.</ref> These movements vary in range of motion, speed and intensity and have a gradual onset and end. Writhing movements wax and wane and give the appearance of being fluid and graceful. At around 9 weeks, fidgety movements replace writhing. Fidgety movements usually continue until around 16 to 20 weeks old.<ref name=":1" /> Fidgety movements are characterised by small movements of moderate speed with variable acceleration of the legs, neck and trunk in all directions.<ref>Hadders‐Algra M. [https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/dmcn.13540 Neural substrate and clinical significance of general movements: an update.] Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology. 2018 Jan;60(1):39-46.</ref><ref name=":0" />


=== Supine ===
* Although simultaneous flexion and extension movements of the fingers are still common, independent movements of the fingers become more prominent.
[[File:PV.jpg|thumb|Supine-ATNR|alt=|200x200px]]
Young infants generally hold their head rotated to one side when lying supine. When the head moves into rotation, the [[asymmetrical tonic neck reflex]] (ATNR) might occur (see figure). However, this reflex is not obligatory. By the end of the 1 to 2 month period, infants are more likely to hold their head in the midline when lying supine and can easily turn to either side to explore their environment. Infants can then track an object from the side to the midline, but not across the midline. They can also track an object in the downwards direction.<ref name=":1" />


==== General and fidgety movements ====
General movements continue to be characterised by writhing movements that involve the head, trunk and extremities in the 1-2 month period. Writhing general movements in the healthy full term infant are described as complex and involve the entire body, notably arm, leg, neck, and trunk movements in variable sequence. They wax and they wane varying in intensity and speed, range of motion, and have a gradual onset and a gradual end. However, towards the end of this period fidgety movements (FMs) are increasingly present. <ref name=":0">Einspieler C, Marschik PB, Prechtl HF. [https://www.researchgate.net/publication/247399672_Human_Motor_Behavior_Prenatal_Origin_and_Early_Postnatal_Development Human motor behavior: Prenatal origin and early postnatal development.] Zeitschrift für Psychologie/Journal of Psychology. 2008;216(3):147.</ref>


FMs are small movements of moderate speed with variable acceleration of the neck, trunk, and limbs in all directions. They may appear as early as six weeks after term, but usually occur from around 9 weeks until 16–20 weeks, occasionally even a few weeks longer. They fade out when antigravity and intentional movements start to dominate. 


=== Prone ===
The presence and character of fidgety movements are good indicators of the integrity of the infant's nervous system.<ref name=":0" />
[[File:Pronebaby.jpg|left|thumb|Prone playing]]
==='''Head control and neck movements'''===
Infants in the 1 to 2 month period can briefly lift their heads up when in a prone position. Their feet remain in dorsiflexion, increasing as they flex their hips and knees, and decreasing as they extend their lower extremities. Kicking can be observed in a prone position. Infants may kick one or both legs. Unilateral kicking is associated with a lateral weight shift in weight-bearing and a lateral flexion of the trunk.<ref name=":1" />
At the beginning of the 1-2 month period the infant still tends to lie supine with the head turned to one or the other side. Head rotation continues to be associated with some neck extension and lateral rotation.  Head turning may be associated with an asymmetrical tonic neck reflex (ATNR) posture, but this is not obligatory.
[[File:Pamv9.png|center|thumb|400x400px]]


By the end of the 1-2 month period the infant is more inclined to hold the head in the midline and easily turns the head to look at interesting objects and events in the environment.
[[File:Pamv2.png|thumb|233x233px|alt=|center]]


==== Rolling ====
At this age infants tend to lie with the upper extremities (UEs) abducted and extended, a position that helps to stabilize the trunk and provide a stable base for head movements and kicking.<ref name=":1">Bly L. [https://www.semanticscholar.org/paper/Motor-Skills-Acquisition-in-the-First-Year%3A-An-to-Bly/73bd3e94f6dd83e707be95704e920bd9304fa6bb Motor skills acquisition in the first year: An illustrated guide to normal development]. Psychological Corporation; 1994.</ref>
Many infants do not like being placed in prone and quickly get upset and roll from prone to supine. As described below, rolling is initiated in one of two ways (the following describe an infant rolling onto their left side). The infant:[[File:Rolling.jpg|thumb|400x400px|Rolling prone to supine]]
[[File:Pamv3.png|thumb|214x214px|alt=|center]]
* Lifts their head
* Extends their thoracic spine
* Extends the hips and knees
* Shifts their centre of mass caudally
* Retracts their right shoulder
* Flexes their right hip
* Falls onto their back<ref name=":1" />


'''OR'''
The infant is also able to combine neck rotation with extension of the head to look upwards.


* Lifts their head
However, control of the exact position of the head is clearly still developing, as rotation is usually associated with some neck extension and lateral flexion. This combination of movements suggests that the movement is brought about by contraction of the sternocleidomastoid muscles, with limited action in the deep neck stabilisers.<ref name=":1" />
* Pushes down with their right hand
[[File:Pamv4.png|thumb|229x229px|alt=|center]]
* Extends their elbow
* Extends their thoracic spine
* Shifts their centre of mass caudally
* Falls onto back<ref name=":1" />


In the second movement sequence, there is no associated hip extension; the hips remain flexed throughout.<ref name=":1" />
The 1-2 month old infant is able to visually follow an object from the side to the midline, as well as follow an object moved in a downwards direction. The ability to follow an object across the midline is not present
[[File:Pamv5.png|thumb|500x500px|alt=|center]]


Weight-bearing through the hands in prone is seen at the end of the 1 to 2 month period. Extension of the hips is seen with thoracic and head extension. Knee movement does not always couple with hip movement, which allows disassociation and freedom for each joint.<ref name=":1" />
=== Upper extremity (UE) actions ===
=== Reaching and Grasping ===
During periods of relative quiet the one-month-old infant adopts a variety of postures of the upper extremities. Abduction of the shoulders with the upper arms resting on the support surface is often observed – in this position the UEs serve as outriggers and help to stabilize the trunk when moving the head and lower extremities. Interestingly this co-opting of the UEs for a postural function during this period is associated with a decrease in the occurrence of swiping actions towards objects within reach.<ref name=":2">von Hofsten C. [https://www.researchgate.net/publication/232516582_Hofsten_C_Developmental_changes_in_the_organization_of_prereaching_movements_Developmental_Psychology_203_378-388 Developmental changes in the organization of prereaching movements]. Developmental psychology. 1984 May;20(3):378.</ref>  
[[File:Pv.jpg|thumb|alt=|272x272px|Infant reaching for a toy]]
Infants in the 1 to 2 month period will reach for a toy by making large swiping motions with their elbow and fingers extended. Predominately, their hands do not make contact with the object. As they move through this period, extension of the fingers becomes less pronounced and small shoulder and elbow movements help facilitate grasping the toy. At around 10-12 weeks old, the infant's ability to steady their head and trunk while moving their upper extremities becomes more consistent. This allows them to reach for fixed or suspended toys.  Reaching at this point becomes more goal directed.<ref name=":1" /> <ref>Corbetta D, Wiener RF, Thurman SL. [https://books.google.com/books?hl=en&lr=&id=wmAPEAAAQBAJ&oi=fnd&pg=PA18&dq=newborn+reaching+and+grasping&ots=IE-M9-3X6Z&sig=Sx9_DQdIXlaOHNQI96P-GnZyrcQ#v=onepage&q=newborn%20reaching%20and%20grasping&f=false Learning to reach in infancy]. Reach-to-Grasp Behavior. 2018 Aug 28:18-41.</ref> Flexion and extension of the fingers are often seen as infants interact with various surfaces they encounter.<ref>DiMercurio A, Connell JP, Clark M, Corbetta D. [https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02613/full A naturalistic observation of spontaneous touches to the body and environment in the first 2 months of life]. Frontiers in psychology. 2018:2613.</ref><ref name=":1" /> Exploring toys with their hands and fingers permits them to learn about the behaviour, structure and texture of an object.<ref name=":1" />


=== Kicking ===
Head rotation may be associated with the fencing position (with extension of the elbow on the side to which the face is turned). However this association decreases over the 1-2 month period, and importantly, is never obligatory.
[[File:Babykicking.jpg|thumb|Unilateral kicking|alt=|234x234px]]
Infants in the 1 to 2 month period have a physiological flexor stiffness at the knees and hips.  By 10 weeks, the infant will gain full lower extremity range of motion and is able to extend the hip and knee with an associated anterior pelvic tilt. Single or alternating leg kicking can be seen during this time with hip and knee motions coupled. The feet will occasionally push down on a surface, causing lateral trunk weight shift with trunk and head extension. The feet will demonstrate increased plantarflexion during this time as well.<ref name=":1" />


The tendency to abduct the shoulders and use the UEs as outriggers decreases towards the end of this period when infants start to bring their hands into the midline.
[[File:Pamv6.png|center|thumb|500x500px]]


At the beginning of the 1-2 month period infants produce large range swiping movements of the upper extremities. These swiping movements are associated with elbow extension and extension of the fingers. The hand comes close to the object, but mostly does not make contact.
[[File:Pamv7.png|center|thumb|500x500px]]


=== Pull to Sit ===
Over the coming weeks the infant gains more control over reaching movements and starts to reach towards objects within easy reach with greater success. The extension of the fingers seen in the one month old infant become less pronounced<ref name=":2" />  
[[File:Pulltosit.jpg|thumb|alt=|Pull to sit]]
Infants can pull to sit keeping their head in line with their trunk when they are 1-2 months old. As they anticipate being pulled up, they can activate their trunk and neck flexor muscles while stiffening their hips and upper extremities. Once upright, these infants are able to move their head to gaze at the person lifting them.<ref name=":1" />


The infant brings the hand into the proximity of the toy and then uses small range movements of the shoulder and elbow to explore different ways of touching and grasping the toy.


Towards the end of this period (10-12 weeks), as the infant's ability to steady the head and trunk when moving the UEs becomes more reliable the infant gains more control of reaching towards toys.
They are able to bring the hand into contact with the toy and start to use the fingers to explore it.
This is the start of the ability to stabilise the position of the hand in space and at the same time use independent finger movements to gather information about the texture, structure and behaviour of objects. Visual attention to the toy and the hand provides further information that starts to link what is felt and seen.
[[File:Pamv8.png|thumb|400x400px|alt=|center]]
=== Exploratory hand movements ===
During this period infants continue to use their hands to gather information about the surfaces they encounter. Contact with a surface is often associated with exploratory movements of the hand across the surface, or repeated flexion and extension of the fingers.
Independent movements of the fingers are also frequently seen, especially when the infant is socially engaged or is paying attention to an object within reaching distance.
=== Postural sway and postural stability ===
Observing an infant during periods of quiet supine lying allows one to observe the postural sway present in the trunk. These exploratory movements allow the postural system to gather the sensory information needed for estimating the position of the body as a whole and exploring the most effective strategies to maintain a stable posture.<ref>Dusing SC, Thacker LR, Stergiou N, Galloway JC. Early complexity supports development of motor behaviors in the first months of life. Developmental Psychobiology. 2013;55(4):404-14.</ref>
Here you see Will lying quietly at 5 and 10 weeks of age. The upper arms come to rest on the support surface and the continuous lateral sway of the trunk provides changing contact between the upper arm and the support surface.
Will is also exploring the contact between his feet and the support surface, and between each other. This exploration is important for developing the infant's ability to use surfaces to support their actions.
[[File:Pamv9.png|thumb|alt=|center]]
=== Lower extremity (LE) movements ===
The 1-2 month old infant engages in periods of relative calm when the feet rest on the support surface with varying amounts of flexion of the hips and knees.
At 7 weeks Will's range of hip and knee extension is still restricted by the remaining physiological flexor stiffness.
[[File:Pamv10.png|center|thumb|500x500px]]
By 10 weeks Will has close to full range of hip and knee extension. Full hip extension is associated with anterior pelvic tilt.
The range of ankle plantar flexion has also increased.
[[File:Pamv11.png|thumb|225x225px|alt=|center]]
The 1-2 month old still engages in extended periods of kicking. Movement patterns include repeated single leg kicking with alternate leg kicking and bilateral hip and knee flexion and extension.
At this age hip and knee movements are still coupled. The ankles remain in dorsiflexion with intermittent flexion and extension of the toes.
[[File:Pamv12.png|thumb|400x400px|alt=|center]]
The range of movement of hip flexion and knee extension during kicking movements increases over the 1-2 month period, as seen in these pictures of Will at 10 weeks.
=== Lower Extremity Bridging ===
From time to time one or both feet push down on the SS. Pushing down with one foot is associated with head and trunk extension and lateral weight shift.
[[File:Pamv13.png|thumb|400x400px|alt=|center]]
=== Foot reaching ===
Although spontaneous kicking actions tend to still show strong intra-limb coupling, research has shown that 2 month old infants can adapt their LE actions to operate a mobile and make contact with a toy that has been suspended in the midline.
=== Pull-to-sit ===
The infant's response to the pull-to-sit manoeuvre is often used as a test when assessing motor development. It provides a good measure of the infant's neck muscle strength as well as the development of effective anticipatory postural responses.
By the end of the 1-2 month period, infants have learned to anticipate being lifted and will participate in the PTS manoeuvre by engaging the neck and trunk flexor muscles, stiffening the UEs and flexing the hips.
The head is held in line with the trunk as the shoulders are lifted.
[[File:Pamv14.png|center|thumb|700x700px]]
Once in the upright position the head is held erect, and the infant is able to lift the face to look at the person who has pulled him into sitting.
[[File:Pamv15.png|thumb|400x400px|alt=|center]]


=== Sitting ===
=== Sitting ===
[[File:Babysitting.jpg|thumb|Sitting with hip support]]
Bly (1994)<ref name=":1" /> describes the characteristics of sitting at the beginning of the 1-2 month period as follows:
Infants in the 1 to 2 month period will need support in sitting. Trunk support in sitting has the following characteristics:<ref name=":1" />
 
* Head falls forward
* In supported sitting the head falls forwards although the infant does make brief attempts to hold it up.
* Brief period of scapular retraction
* The infant's UEs no longer hang loosely at the infant's side.
* Increased flexion of the elbows
* During brief periods of lifting the head there is retraction of the scapulae, increased flexion of the elbows, forearm pronation, extension of the wrist and flexion of the finger.
* Forearm pronation
* Bly surmises that the retraction of the scapulae may provide synergistic stability for head lifting.
* Extension of the wrist
 
* Flexion of the fingers
* The hips are flexed, abducted and laterally rotated, the knees more flexed than in the neonatal period, and the ankles are strongly dorsiflexed and everted as in the neonatal period.
* Feet flexed and everted
Over the 1-2 month period the infants gain increasing control of the position of the trunk when they sit with support around the hips.
 
They are able to maintain the trunk in a semi-erect position, with the line of gravity falling anterior the flexion-extension axis of the hips, creating a gravity flexion moment at the hips which is counteracted by the hips extensors. The trunk and neck extensors work to maintain some extension in the trunk.
 
[[File:Pamv16.png|thumb|400x400px|alt=|center]]
 
The ability to control the position of the trunk is less well developed when the trunk is tilted backwards so that the LOG falls posterior to the flexion/extension axis of the hips,
 
Notice how this rapid backwards movement initiates a response in the trunk and neck flexors, as well as reactions in his upper limbs as the postural system responds to the rapid backwards movement of the head.
 
[[File:Pamv17.png|thumb|450x450px|alt=|center]]
 
=== Response to being picked up and moved ===
By the end of the 1-2 month period infants have started to anticipate being picked up and moved.
 
They are able to hold the head in line with the trunk when supported around the chest, especially when tipped forwards or laterally.
[[File:Pamv18.png|thumb|450x450px|alt=|center]]
 
=== Prone <ref>Hewitt L, Kerr E, Stanley RM, Okely AD. [https://watermark.silverchair.com/peds_20192168.pdf?token=AQECAHi208BE49Ooan9kkhW_Ercy7Dm3ZL_9Cf3qfKAc485ysgAAAqAwggKcBgkqhkiG9w0BBwagggKNMIICiQIBADCCAoIGCSqGSIb3DQEHATAeBglghkgBZQMEAS4wEQQMwq4dtML7QW324ERfAgEQgIICU78NJsc4-J-iLB218tvV3kg-x65PS-k5XM5WiPj48-OKz5KXKNjAE3Tix8LzG6q5tOtgbJyT8u9-_4yVdYeD2f-TUVuGisqSp4DCUg7s1TnCXgcxjey3yGHyHwNwTAXXi3GrjGDxiBPpcrmyfVEMadupjw5DoJdknOmoOdXzAPDNtUxTzYl-rneTD1lcf95PKRYEsIcbl0HfhtrD-g_bCNjyxDJvj_NkSeQInpwWFuVlrshC3-o95b09gucq-dnRtlPQW3wlLbx2jCjN0bWihg61AlD1K6W3_P2rblEpb2f3XgOFEhjjGTG5ZBevdh4N0aZbKlp9ojOzXav2ua45ZhOBl_lSt47JuUCN5ODZnpiIcfvdhMr1bC7yjQlY7K9KcWKhQgHK_GbXITne-oM2LHX8EBIXBj6FidROWYYS-Z-_HFdL9MtFSjyz9QohIacT5qnBs0KsNWieivU8bE0OF5fufFIfPuEz9g6ZBJWr4KP4YEjp4B6zpVQeMmp1_uYvYwifI1NkTpj1-3N3TWN1EcbRh8_0IVz4yeRyj_CKVtm2n1A6oR5qSmybFpOd1vMCzc8fvXDuIGF8nqGal9bgpPkpsOhDuISDPBU4uRfIxQ7L9GS1PsajPLjsZzOOmBcfyYhXUPL-PsFICEfyfgievSuvf5zsM9ZsoTZBgccXENgdpKsJIrXBikiKCE8oxemspnhDaxNCIx1l6MQ3okFWcbcF9V5XSL-X2DCnKXAlwZkggMuSuZMGaidrozPug-vXgsrMgFZLaNDiDqYeM9XeYK5_PV8 Tummy Time and Infant Health Outcomes: A Systematic Review.] Pediatrics. 2020 Jun;145(6):e20192168. </ref> ===
'''Back to sleep advice'''. In the past many infants were put into prone for sleeping, but more recently because prone sleeping is a risk factor for sudden infant death, parents are advised to let infants sleep on their backs. As a result many infants do not like being placed in prone, quickly start to cry and also quickly learn to roll from prone to supine.
 
At the beginning of the 1-2 month period, infants start to lift the head up off the support surface (SS) for brief periods of time. This is accomplished by the neck and thoracic extensor muscle activity.
[[File:Pamv19.png|thumb|alt=|center]]
 
When in prone infants engage in active kicking, alternating between unilateral and bilateral hip and knee flexion and extension. The ankles remain dorsiflexed, with the range of dorsiflexion (DF) increasing with full hip and knee flexion and decreasing with hip and knee extension.
 
Unilateral kicking movements are associated with lateral flexion of the trunk with a lateral shift in weight bearing. Bilateral hip and knee flexion is associated with anterior pelvic tilt and a cephalad shift of weight onto the upper torso.
[[File:Pamv20.png|thumb|500x500px|alt=|center]]
 
=== Rolling from prone to supine ===
Will's ability to initiate rolling from prone to supine illustrate how by 5 weeks he has learned to exploit his rapidly developing control of movement of the extremities to initiate a sequence of movements directed towards achieving a goal.
 
After a period of kicking, Will decides that he has had enough of being in prone, and initiates rolling to supine by lifting his head and extending the thoracic spine. These movements are associated with a more extended position of the hips and knees bilaterally and a shift of his centre of mass (COM) caudally.
 
He rotates his shoulder girdle back on the right, flexes the right hip "falls" onto his back.
[[File:Pamv21.png|center|thumb|800x800px]]
 
 
 
I turn Will back into prone, and he immediately rolls back into supine, this time using a slightly different pattern of movement. He pushes down on his right hand, extends his elbow, and extends the thoracic spine. The COM shifts caudally, but there is no associated extension of the hips. Will collapses onto his left side with the hips still in flexion, and then rolls onto his back.
[[File:Pamv22.png|center|thumb|800x800px]]
 
By the end of the 1-2 month period, the infant's ability to lift the head and extend the thoracic spine has improved. This is associated with taking some weight on the hands.
 
Extension of the neck and thoracic spine is now associated with extension and adduction of the hips.


At around 6 weeks, infants can extend their thoracic spine and head when supported around their waist. When they are closer to 2 months, infants can maintain a semi-erect position with support at their hips. This facilitates their neck and trunk extensors.<ref name=":1" />
The ROM of extension at the hips and knees has increased. Some of the time flexion of the knee is associated with flexion of the hip, but not always. This is the beginning of uncoupling of movements the hip and knee flexion and the beginning of the ability to dissociate movements of the two joints.
[[File:Pamv23.png|thumb|500x500px|alt=|center]]


== Resources ==
== Resources ==
Line 132: Line 224:
== References ==
== References ==
<references />
<references />
[[Category:Course pages]]
[[Category:Rehabilitation]]
[[Category:Rehabilitation]]
[[Category:ReLAB Content Development Project]]
[[Category:ReLAB-HS Course Page]]
[[Category:Paediatrics]]
[[Category:Paediatrics]]
[[Category:Course Pages]]

Latest revision as of 07:36, 25 September 2023

Infant development: 1-2 month period[edit | edit source]

This is an exciting period as it the time when infants’ perceptual-motor behaviour shifts from spontaneous movements to movements that become increasingly intentional as the infant uses longer periods of being awake and alert to explore and connect with their social and physical environment.

  • When awake and alert infants alternate between period of activity and periods of quiet
  • Infants gain greater control of their head posture and movements, increasing their ability for visual reach and information gathering
  • They look towards interesting sounds and visual events in the environment
  • They actively explore their bodies, clothing and surrounding surfaces with their hands and their feet
  • They are becoming increasingly self-aware and develop an awareness of their own bodies relative to the surrounding physical environment
  • Reaching becomes more intentional and successful.

Communication and Social Behaviour[edit | edit source]

During this period infants’ communication and social behaviour increases and become more complex, sustained and expressive.[1]

  • First smiles occur
  • They turn their heads towards voices and other sounds in the environment
  • Quieten their limb movements or smile in response to sounds
  • Show increasing interest in faces and recognise familiar faces
  • Make eye contact
  • Cry differently for different needs (e.g. hungry vs tired)
  • Produce a range of pre-speech sounds – known as protophones - that include grunts, coos and gurgles.

Vocalisation[edit | edit source]

During this period infants produce a range of pre-speech sounds, known as protophones.[2]

  • These sounds include grunts, squeals, coos, and vowel like sounds.
  • Vocalisations becomes more extended and include changes in emotional tone (known as inflexion)
  • Vocal behavior is often produced during social interaction but is also heard when infants are not attending to the environment.

This spontaneous vocalisation allows the infant to practice and explore different ways of producing a range of different sounds which provides the foundation for later babbling and words. [3]

Social interaction and mirroring[edit | edit source]

In the 1-2 month period infants continue to pay special attention to faces. They also start to recognise familiar faces and will smile at them. Their ability to sustain social interaction increases and they are figuring out the process of turn taking which is an inherent aspect of a conversation. Adult social partners will often imitate infants’ facial expressions, lip and jaw movements and vocalisations which act as a mirror that allows the infant to “see” and “hear” copies of their own actions.

When speaking to infants, adults typically alter the acoustic properties of their speech in a variety of ways compared with how they speak to other adults; for example, they use higher pitch, increased pitch range that is making the voice go up and down in tone, more pitch variability, and slower speech rate. Recent research by Tanya Broesch and Gregory Bryant has shown that these vocal changes happen similarly, not only across industrialised populations but also in traditional societies.[4]

Spontaneous touches[edit | edit source]

During this period infants continue to use their hands to gather information about the surfaces they encounter. Contact with a surface is often associated with exploratory movements of the hand across the surface, or repeated flexion and extension of the fingers and grasping. Independent movements of the fingers are also frequently seen, especially when the infant is socially engaged or is paying attention to an object within reaching distance. [5]

Perceptual-Motor Development[6][edit | edit source]

  • During the 1-2 month period the infant is awake and alert for longer periods of time, increasingly responds to sounds and sights from the environment and gains more control of movements of the head and limbs
  • Head control in supine is improving as the infant learns to hold the head steady in the midline as well as turn the head to locate interesting sights and sounds.
  • When held upright infants can keep the head erect for extended periods of time.
  • Neck rotation is still associated with extension and lateral flexion.
  • Spontaneous movements of the lower extremities, along with the pull of gravity serve to stretch the hip and knee flexor muscles, leading to increased range of extension of the extremity joints.
  • Postural control of the trunk in supine is improving but movement of the extremities still leads to lateral displacement and sometimes rotation of the pelvis.
  • Vigorous kicking of the lower extremities (LEs) is often associated with abduction of the shoulders and extension of the elbows as the infant works to maintain a steady torso.
  • Kicking movements still show strong coupling between the hip and knee joints, however the ability to uncouple hip and knee emerges when the infant engages in goal directed actions of the foot, such as reaching to a target with the foot.
  • Whole body general movements give way to fidgety general movements from around 9 weeks.
  • Infants start to reach successfully towards interesting objects within reach and start to use finger movements to explore objects.
  • Infants actively explore surfaces with their feet and hands.
  • Although simultaneous flexion and extension movements of the fingers are still common, independent movements of the fingers become more prominent.

General and fidgety movements[edit | edit source]

General movements continue to be characterised by writhing movements that involve the head, trunk and extremities in the 1-2 month period. Writhing general movements in the healthy full term infant are described as complex and involve the entire body, notably arm, leg, neck, and trunk movements in variable sequence. They wax and they wane varying in intensity and speed, range of motion, and have a gradual onset and a gradual end. However, towards the end of this period fidgety movements (FMs) are increasingly present. [7]

FMs are small movements of moderate speed with variable acceleration of the neck, trunk, and limbs in all directions. They may appear as early as six weeks after term, but usually occur from around 9 weeks until 16–20 weeks, occasionally even a few weeks longer. They fade out when antigravity and intentional movements start to dominate.

The presence and character of fidgety movements are good indicators of the integrity of the infant's nervous system.[7]

Head control and neck movements[edit | edit source]

At the beginning of the 1-2 month period the infant still tends to lie supine with the head turned to one or the other side. Head rotation continues to be associated with some neck extension and lateral rotation. Head turning may be associated with an asymmetrical tonic neck reflex (ATNR) posture, but this is not obligatory.

Pamv9.png

By the end of the 1-2 month period the infant is more inclined to hold the head in the midline and easily turns the head to look at interesting objects and events in the environment.

At this age infants tend to lie with the upper extremities (UEs) abducted and extended, a position that helps to stabilize the trunk and provide a stable base for head movements and kicking.[8]

The infant is also able to combine neck rotation with extension of the head to look upwards.

However, control of the exact position of the head is clearly still developing, as rotation is usually associated with some neck extension and lateral flexion. This combination of movements suggests that the movement is brought about by contraction of the sternocleidomastoid muscles, with limited action in the deep neck stabilisers.[8]

The 1-2 month old infant is able to visually follow an object from the side to the midline, as well as follow an object moved in a downwards direction. The ability to follow an object across the midline is not present

Upper extremity (UE) actions[edit | edit source]

During periods of relative quiet the one-month-old infant adopts a variety of postures of the upper extremities. Abduction of the shoulders with the upper arms resting on the support surface is often observed – in this position the UEs serve as outriggers and help to stabilize the trunk when moving the head and lower extremities. Interestingly this co-opting of the UEs for a postural function during this period is associated with a decrease in the occurrence of swiping actions towards objects within reach.[9]

Head rotation may be associated with the fencing position (with extension of the elbow on the side to which the face is turned). However this association decreases over the 1-2 month period, and importantly, is never obligatory.

The tendency to abduct the shoulders and use the UEs as outriggers decreases towards the end of this period when infants start to bring their hands into the midline.

Pamv6.png

At the beginning of the 1-2 month period infants produce large range swiping movements of the upper extremities. These swiping movements are associated with elbow extension and extension of the fingers. The hand comes close to the object, but mostly does not make contact.

Pamv7.png

Over the coming weeks the infant gains more control over reaching movements and starts to reach towards objects within easy reach with greater success. The extension of the fingers seen in the one month old infant become less pronounced[9]

The infant brings the hand into the proximity of the toy and then uses small range movements of the shoulder and elbow to explore different ways of touching and grasping the toy.

Towards the end of this period (10-12 weeks), as the infant's ability to steady the head and trunk when moving the UEs becomes more reliable the infant gains more control of reaching towards toys.

They are able to bring the hand into contact with the toy and start to use the fingers to explore it.

This is the start of the ability to stabilise the position of the hand in space and at the same time use independent finger movements to gather information about the texture, structure and behaviour of objects. Visual attention to the toy and the hand provides further information that starts to link what is felt and seen.

Exploratory hand movements[edit | edit source]

During this period infants continue to use their hands to gather information about the surfaces they encounter. Contact with a surface is often associated with exploratory movements of the hand across the surface, or repeated flexion and extension of the fingers.

Independent movements of the fingers are also frequently seen, especially when the infant is socially engaged or is paying attention to an object within reaching distance.

Postural sway and postural stability[edit | edit source]

Observing an infant during periods of quiet supine lying allows one to observe the postural sway present in the trunk. These exploratory movements allow the postural system to gather the sensory information needed for estimating the position of the body as a whole and exploring the most effective strategies to maintain a stable posture.[10]

Here you see Will lying quietly at 5 and 10 weeks of age. The upper arms come to rest on the support surface and the continuous lateral sway of the trunk provides changing contact between the upper arm and the support surface.

Will is also exploring the contact between his feet and the support surface, and between each other. This exploration is important for developing the infant's ability to use surfaces to support their actions.

Lower extremity (LE) movements[edit | edit source]

The 1-2 month old infant engages in periods of relative calm when the feet rest on the support surface with varying amounts of flexion of the hips and knees.

At 7 weeks Will's range of hip and knee extension is still restricted by the remaining physiological flexor stiffness.

Pamv10.png

By 10 weeks Will has close to full range of hip and knee extension. Full hip extension is associated with anterior pelvic tilt.

The range of ankle plantar flexion has also increased.

The 1-2 month old still engages in extended periods of kicking. Movement patterns include repeated single leg kicking with alternate leg kicking and bilateral hip and knee flexion and extension.

At this age hip and knee movements are still coupled. The ankles remain in dorsiflexion with intermittent flexion and extension of the toes.

The range of movement of hip flexion and knee extension during kicking movements increases over the 1-2 month period, as seen in these pictures of Will at 10 weeks.

Lower Extremity Bridging[edit | edit source]

From time to time one or both feet push down on the SS. Pushing down with one foot is associated with head and trunk extension and lateral weight shift.

Foot reaching[edit | edit source]

Although spontaneous kicking actions tend to still show strong intra-limb coupling, research has shown that 2 month old infants can adapt their LE actions to operate a mobile and make contact with a toy that has been suspended in the midline.

Pull-to-sit[edit | edit source]

The infant's response to the pull-to-sit manoeuvre is often used as a test when assessing motor development. It provides a good measure of the infant's neck muscle strength as well as the development of effective anticipatory postural responses.

By the end of the 1-2 month period, infants have learned to anticipate being lifted and will participate in the PTS manoeuvre by engaging the neck and trunk flexor muscles, stiffening the UEs and flexing the hips.

The head is held in line with the trunk as the shoulders are lifted.

Pamv14.png

Once in the upright position the head is held erect, and the infant is able to lift the face to look at the person who has pulled him into sitting.

Sitting[edit | edit source]

Bly (1994)[8] describes the characteristics of sitting at the beginning of the 1-2 month period as follows:

  • In supported sitting the head falls forwards although the infant does make brief attempts to hold it up.
  • The infant's UEs no longer hang loosely at the infant's side.
  • During brief periods of lifting the head there is retraction of the scapulae, increased flexion of the elbows, forearm pronation, extension of the wrist and flexion of the finger.
  • Bly surmises that the retraction of the scapulae may provide synergistic stability for head lifting.
  • The hips are flexed, abducted and laterally rotated, the knees more flexed than in the neonatal period, and the ankles are strongly dorsiflexed and everted as in the neonatal period.

Over the 1-2 month period the infants gain increasing control of the position of the trunk when they sit with support around the hips.

They are able to maintain the trunk in a semi-erect position, with the line of gravity falling anterior the flexion-extension axis of the hips, creating a gravity flexion moment at the hips which is counteracted by the hips extensors. The trunk and neck extensors work to maintain some extension in the trunk.

The ability to control the position of the trunk is less well developed when the trunk is tilted backwards so that the LOG falls posterior to the flexion/extension axis of the hips,

Notice how this rapid backwards movement initiates a response in the trunk and neck flexors, as well as reactions in his upper limbs as the postural system responds to the rapid backwards movement of the head.

Response to being picked up and moved[edit | edit source]

By the end of the 1-2 month period infants have started to anticipate being picked up and moved.

They are able to hold the head in line with the trunk when supported around the chest, especially when tipped forwards or laterally.

Prone [11][edit | edit source]

Back to sleep advice. In the past many infants were put into prone for sleeping, but more recently because prone sleeping is a risk factor for sudden infant death, parents are advised to let infants sleep on their backs. As a result many infants do not like being placed in prone, quickly start to cry and also quickly learn to roll from prone to supine.

At the beginning of the 1-2 month period, infants start to lift the head up off the support surface (SS) for brief periods of time. This is accomplished by the neck and thoracic extensor muscle activity.

When in prone infants engage in active kicking, alternating between unilateral and bilateral hip and knee flexion and extension. The ankles remain dorsiflexed, with the range of dorsiflexion (DF) increasing with full hip and knee flexion and decreasing with hip and knee extension.

Unilateral kicking movements are associated with lateral flexion of the trunk with a lateral shift in weight bearing. Bilateral hip and knee flexion is associated with anterior pelvic tilt and a cephalad shift of weight onto the upper torso.

Rolling from prone to supine[edit | edit source]

Will's ability to initiate rolling from prone to supine illustrate how by 5 weeks he has learned to exploit his rapidly developing control of movement of the extremities to initiate a sequence of movements directed towards achieving a goal.

After a period of kicking, Will decides that he has had enough of being in prone, and initiates rolling to supine by lifting his head and extending the thoracic spine. These movements are associated with a more extended position of the hips and knees bilaterally and a shift of his centre of mass (COM) caudally.

He rotates his shoulder girdle back on the right, flexes the right hip "falls" onto his back.

Pamv21.png


I turn Will back into prone, and he immediately rolls back into supine, this time using a slightly different pattern of movement. He pushes down on his right hand, extends his elbow, and extends the thoracic spine. The COM shifts caudally, but there is no associated extension of the hips. Will collapses onto his left side with the hips still in flexion, and then rolls onto his back.

Pamv22.png

By the end of the 1-2 month period, the infant's ability to lift the head and extend the thoracic spine has improved. This is associated with taking some weight on the hands.

Extension of the neck and thoracic spine is now associated with extension and adduction of the hips.

The ROM of extension at the hips and knees has increased. Some of the time flexion of the knee is associated with flexion of the hip, but not always. This is the beginning of uncoupling of movements the hip and knee flexion and the beginning of the ability to dissociate movements of the two joints.

Resources[edit | edit source]

References[edit | edit source]

  1. Farran LK, Yoo H, Lee CC, Bowman DD, Oller DK. Temporal Coordination in Mother-Infant Vocal Interaction: A Cross-Cultural Comparison. Front Psychol. 2019 Nov 8;10:2374.
  2. Oller DK, Ramsay G, Bene E, Long HL, Griebel U. Protophones, the precursors to speech, dominate the human infant vocal landscape. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2021 Oct 25;376(1836):20200255.
  3. Long HL, Bowman DD, Yoo H, Burkhardt-Reed MM, Bene ER, Oller DK. Social and endogenous infant vocalizations. PLoS One. 2020 Aug 5;15(8):e0224956.
  4. Broesch TL, Bryant GA. Prosody in infant-directed speech is similar across western and traditional cultures. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2015 Jan 1;16(1):31-43.
  5. DiMercurio A, Connell JP, Clark M, Corbetta D. A naturalistic observation of spontaneous touches to the body and environment in the first 2 months of life. Frontiers in psychology. 2018 Dec 18;9:2613.
  6. Baek S, Jaffe-Dax S, Emberson LL. How an infant's active response to structured experience supports perceptual-cognitive development. Prog Brain Res. 2020;254:167-186.
  7. 7.0 7.1 Einspieler C, Marschik PB, Prechtl HF. Human motor behavior: Prenatal origin and early postnatal development. Zeitschrift für Psychologie/Journal of Psychology. 2008;216(3):147.
  8. 8.0 8.1 8.2 Bly L. Motor skills acquisition in the first year: An illustrated guide to normal development. Psychological Corporation; 1994.
  9. 9.0 9.1 von Hofsten C. Developmental changes in the organization of prereaching movements. Developmental psychology. 1984 May;20(3):378.
  10. Dusing SC, Thacker LR, Stergiou N, Galloway JC. Early complexity supports development of motor behaviors in the first months of life. Developmental Psychobiology. 2013;55(4):404-14.
  11. Hewitt L, Kerr E, Stanley RM, Okely AD. Tummy Time and Infant Health Outcomes: A Systematic Review. Pediatrics. 2020 Jun;145(6):e20192168.