Perceptual-Motor Abilities of Infants in the 1 to 2 Month Period: Difference between revisions

No edit summary
No edit summary
Line 2: Line 2:
'''Top Contributors''' - {{Special:Contributors/{{FULLPAGENAME}}}}</div>
'''Top Contributors''' - {{Special:Contributors/{{FULLPAGENAME}}}}</div>


== Introduction ==
== Infant development: 1-2 month period ==
During the 1 to 2 month old period infants gain various new skills using their perceptual-motor abilities. Perception involves understanding, categorising and translating sensory information.  Examples of perceptual ability include:
This is an exciting period as it the time when infants’ perceptual-motor behaviour shifts from


* Recognising:
spontaneous movements to movements that become increasingly intentional as the infant uses
** Familiar faces vs. non-familiar faces
** The voice of a family member
* Distinguishing:
** The height of an object
** The colour of an object
** The depth of an object


Motor ability refers to an infant's capacity to control their body movements. Motor abilities encompass all movements of the body including, waving, kicking, reaching, grasping, etc. Perceptual and motor abilities are collaborative as infants use their perceptual skills to decide on the correct motor action to undertake.<ref>California Department of Education. Perceptual and Motor Development Domain. Last reviewed 2021. Accessible at:https://www.cde.ca.gov/sp/cd/re/itf09percmotdev.asp#percdev</ref> 
longer periods of being awake and alert to explore and connect with their social and physical


Initially, an infant's perceptual-motor behaviour is spontaneous. During the 1 to 2 month period, infants transition from spontaneous movement to intentional movements. During this period of development, you can expect to see the following:<ref name=":1">Versfeld P. Perceptual-Motor Abilities in the Infant 1-2 Month Period Course. Physioplus. 2022.</ref>
environment.
* Movements shift from writhing to fidgety
* Greater head control and movements allowing increased ability to visually reach and gather information
* Turning towards sounds and visual events within their environment
* Exploring their bodies, clothing and surrounding surfaces with their hands and feet
* Successful and intentional reaching


== Communication and Social Interaction ==
* When awake and alert infants alternate between period of activity and periods of quiet
[[File:PV3.jpg|thumb|alt=|278x278px|Infant smiling]]
* Infants gain greater control of their head posture and movements, increasing their ability for
Newborns in the 1-2 month period will begin to demonstrate more complex, sustained and expressive social behaviours. Some of the types of communication and social development you might notice in an infant in the 1 to 2 month period are listed below:<ref name=":1" />
* First smiles
* Turns head towards voices and other sounds within the environment
* Quietens their limbs movements
* Smiles in response to sounds


* Interest in faces
visual reach and information gathering
* Recognises familiar faces
* Makes eye contact
* Has different cries for different needs
* Produces pre-speech sounds known as protophones, which includes grunts, coos, and gurgles


=== Infant Vocalisation ===
* They look towards interesting sounds and visual events in the environment
To practise and explore sounds, infants will spontaneously vocalise, producing a range of sounds. Infants can alter these sounds by changing their emotional tone or inflexion. These different sounds will provide the foundation for later babbling and words.  Pre-speech sounds such as grunts, squeals and coos are referred to as protophones.<ref name=":1" />
* They actively explore their bodies, clothing and surrounding surfaces with their hands and


*
their feet


=== Caregiver Interaction ===
* They are becoming increasingly self-aware and develop an awareness of their own bodies
[[File:PV5.jpg|left|alt=|225x225px|thumb|Caregiver-infant interaction]]
Interactions between the caregiver and infant require clear cues from both the infant and the caregiver to facilitate responses to each other. Taking cues from one another allows the infant and caregiver to change or adapt their behaviour in response to the interaction. This mutual interactive environment teaches the infant communication and social interaction.<ref>Chung FF, Wan GH, Kuo SC, Lin KC, Liu HE. [https://bmcpregnancychildbirth.biomedcentral.com/articles/10.1186/s12884-018-1979-7 Mother–infant interaction quality and sense of parenting competence at six months postpartum for first-time mothers in Taiwan: a multiple time series design.] BMC pregnancy and childbirth. 2018 Dec;18(1):1-3.</ref> Caregivers speak to infants in a language called motherese or baby talk. This type of infant directed speech allows adults to alter their acoustic properties to use a higher pitch, increased pitch range and variability, and a slower speech rate. Research shows that these vocal adjustments happen across all populations.<ref name=":1" />


''"Various aspects of motherese also known as infant-directed speech (IDS) have been studied for many years. As it is a widespread phenomenon, it is suspected to play some important roles in infant development. Therefore, our purpose was to provide an update of the evidence accumulated by reviewing all of the empirical or experimental studies that have been published since 1966 on IDS driving factors and impacts. Two databases were screened and 144 relevant studies were retained. General linguistic and prosodic characteristics of IDS were found in a variety of languages, and IDS was not restricted to mothers. IDS varied with factors associated with the caregiver (e.g., cultural, psychological and physiological) and the infant (e.g., reactivity and interactive feedback). IDS promoted infants' affect, attention and language learning. Cognitive aspects of IDS have been widely studied whereas affective ones still need to be developed. However, during interactions, the following two observations were notable: (1) IDS prosody reflects emotional charges and meets infants' preferences, and (2) mother-infant contingency and synchrony are crucial for IDS production and prolongation. Thus, IDS is part of an interactive loop that may play an important role in infants' cognitive and social development."'' <ref>Saint-Georges, C., Chetouani, M., Cassel, R., Apicella, F., Mahdhaoui, A., Muratori, F., Laznik, M. C., & Cohen, D. (2013). [https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0078103 Motherese in interaction: at the cross-road of emotion and cognition? (A systematic review).] PloS one, 8(10), e78103.</ref>
relative to the surrounding physical environment
==== Mirroring ====
Caregivers will often "mirror" or imitate newborns' facial expressions, jaw and lip movements and vocalisations. This allows the infant to hear and see copies of their own activity.<ref name=":1" /> These parental responses are both instinctive and functionally important as they help the infant increase their social communication skills.<ref>Murray L, Bozicevic L, Ferrari PF, Vaillancourt K, Dalton L, Goodacre T, Chakrabarti B, Bicknell S, Cooper P, Stein A, De Pascalis L. [https://www.cde.ca.gov/sp/cd/re/itf09percmotdev.asp#percdev The effects of maternal mirroring on the development of infant social expressiveness: The case of infant cleft lip]. Neural plasticity. 2018 Dec 17;2018.</ref>


<nowiki>**</nowiki> Infants begin to recognise familiar faces and smile at them during the 1 to 2 month period.
* Reaching becomes more intentional and successful.


Watch this video by Reach Out and Read to see how caregivers and infants communicate: 
=== Communication and social behaviour ===
During this period infants’ communication and social behaviour increases and become more


{{#ev:youtube|O8ETEajtfUs}}
complex, sustained and expressive.


== General Movements ==
* First smiles occur
Within the 1 to 2 month period, infants transition from generalised writhing movements to fidgety movements with an increase in fidgety movements towards the end of this period.<ref name=":2">Einspieler, C., Peharz, R., & Marschik, P. B. (2016). [https://www.scielo.br/j/jped/a/vtz9cCj3PwnGRBphr3Dg8MB/?lang=en Fidgety movements - tiny in appearance, but huge in impact]. Jornal de pediatria, 92(3 Suppl 1), S64–S70. </ref> Writhing movements are characterised by complex, whole body movements including the arm, leg, trunk and neck in variable arrangements.<ref name=":0">Doroniewicz I, Ledwoń DJ, Affanasowicz A, Kieszczyńska K, Latos D, Matyja M, Mitas AW, Myśliwiec A. [https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7660095/#:~:text=A%20large%20percentage%20of%20newborns,analyzed%20group%20of%20healthy%20newborns. Writhing movement detection in newborns on the second and third day of life using pose-based feature machine learning classification]. Sensors. 2020 Jan;20(21):5986.</ref> These movements vary in range of motion, speed and intensity and have a gradual onset and end. Writhing movements wax and wane and give the appearance of being fluid and graceful. At around 9 weeks, fidgety movements replace writhing. Fidgety movements usually continue until around 16 to 20 weeks old.<ref name=":1" /> Fidgety movements are characterised by small movements of moderate speed with variable acceleration of the legs, neck and trunk in all directions.<ref>Hadders‐Algra M. [https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/dmcn.13540 Neural substrate and clinical significance of general movements: an update.] Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology. 2018 Jan;60(1):39-46.</ref><ref name=":0" />The presence and character of fidgety movements are good indicators of the integrity of the infant's nervous system.<ref name=":2" />


=== Supine ===
* They turn their heads towards voices and other sounds in the environment
[[File:PV.jpg|thumb|Supine-ATNR|alt=|200x200px]]
Young infants generally hold their head rotated to one side when lying supine. When the head moves into rotation, the [[asymmetrical tonic neck reflex]] (ATNR) might occur (see figure). However, this reflex is not obligatory. By the end of the 1 to 2 month period, infants are more likely to hold their head in the midline when lying supine and can easily turn to either side to explore their environment. Infants can then track an object from the side to the midline, but not across the midline. They can also track an object in the downwards direction.<ref name=":1" />


* Quieten their limb movements or smile in response to sounds


* Show increasing interest in faces and recognise familiar faces


=== Prone ===
* Make eye contact
[[File:Pronebaby.jpg|left|thumb|Prone playing]]
Infants in the 1 to 2 month period can briefly lift their heads up when in a prone position. Their feet remain in dorsiflexion, increasing as they flex their hips and knees, and decreasing as they extend their lower extremities. Kicking can be observed in a prone position. Infants may kick one or both legs. Unilateral kicking is associated with a lateral weight shift in weight-bearing and a lateral flexion of the trunk.<ref name=":1" />


* Cry differently for different needs (e.g. hungry vs tired)


==== Rolling ====
* Produce a range of pre-speech sounds – known as protophones - that include grunts, coos
Many infants do not like being placed in prone and quickly get upset and roll from prone to supine. As described below, rolling is initiated in one of two ways (the following describe an infant rolling onto their left side). The infant:[[File:Rolling.jpg|thumb|400x400px|Rolling prone to supine]]
* Lifts their head
* Extends their thoracic spine
* Extends the hips and knees
* Shifts their centre of mass caudally
* Retracts their right shoulder
* Flexes their right hip
* Falls onto their back<ref name=":1" />


'''OR'''
and gurgles.


* Lifts their head
=== Vocalisation ===
* Pushes down with their right hand
During this period infants produce a range of pre-speech sounds, known as protophones.
* Extends their elbow
* Extends their thoracic spine
* Shifts their centre of mass caudally
* Falls onto back<ref name=":1" />


In the second movement sequence, there is no associated hip extension; the hips remain flexed throughout.<ref name=":1" />
* These sounds include grunts, squeals, coos, and vowel like sounds.


Weight-bearing through the hands in prone is seen at the end of the 1 to 2 month period. Extension of the hips is seen with thoracic and head extension. Knee movement does not always couple with hip movement, which allows disassociation and freedom for each joint.<ref name=":1" />
* Vocalizations becomes more extended and include changes in emotional tone (known as inflexion)
=== Reaching and Grasping ===
[[File:Pv.jpg|thumb|alt=|272x272px|Infant reaching for a toy]]
Infants in the 1 to 2 month period will reach for a toy by making large swiping motions with their elbow and fingers extended. Predominately, their hands do not make contact with the object. As they move through this period, extension of the fingers becomes less pronounced and small shoulder and elbow movements help facilitate grasping the toy. At around 10-12 weeks old, the infant's ability to steady their head and trunk while moving their upper extremities becomes more consistent. This allows them to reach for fixed or suspended toys.  Reaching at this point becomes more goal directed.<ref name=":1" /> <ref>Corbetta D, Wiener RF, Thurman SL. [https://books.google.com/books?hl=en&lr=&id=wmAPEAAAQBAJ&oi=fnd&pg=PA18&dq=newborn+reaching+and+grasping&ots=IE-M9-3X6Z&sig=Sx9_DQdIXlaOHNQI96P-GnZyrcQ#v=onepage&q=newborn%20reaching%20and%20grasping&f=false Learning to reach in infancy]. Reach-to-Grasp Behavior. 2018 Aug 28:18-41.</ref> Flexion and extension of the fingers are often seen as infants interact with various surfaces they encounter.<ref>DiMercurio A, Connell JP, Clark M, Corbetta D. [https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02613/full A naturalistic observation of spontaneous touches to the body and environment in the first 2 months of life]. Frontiers in psychology. 2018:2613.</ref><ref name=":1" /> Exploring toys with their hands and fingers permits them to learn about the behaviour, structure and texture of an object.<ref name=":1" />


=== Kicking ===
* Vocal behavior is often produced during social interaction but is also heard when infants are not
[[File:Babykicking.jpg|thumb|Unilateral kicking|alt=|234x234px]]
Infants in the 1 to 2 month period have a physiological flexor stiffness at the knees and hips.  By 10 weeks, the infant will gain full lower extremity range of motion and is able to extend the hip and knee with an associated anterior pelvic tilt. Single or alternating leg kicking can be seen during this time with hip and knee motions coupled. The feet will occasionally push down on a surface, causing lateral trunk weight shift with trunk and head extension. The feet will demonstrate increased plantarflexion during this time as well.<ref name=":1" />


attending to the environment.


This spontaneous vocalization allows the infant to practice and explore different ways of producing a


=== Pull to Sit ===
range of different sounds which provides the foundation for later babbling and words.
[[File:Pulltosit.jpg|thumb|alt=|Pull to sit]]
Infants can pull to sit keeping their head in line with their trunk when they are 1-2 months old. As they anticipate being pulled up, they can activate their trunk and neck flexor muscles while stiffening their hips and upper extremities. Once upright, these infants are able to move their head to gaze at the person lifting them.<ref name=":1" />


=== Social interaction and mirroring ===
In the 1-2 month period infants continue to pay special attention to faces. They also start to. recognize familiar faces and will smile at them.  Their ability to sustain social interaction increases and they are figuring out the process of turn taking which is an inherent aspect of a conversation.  Adult social partners will often imitate infants’ facial expressions, lip and jaw movements and vocalizations which act as mirror that allows the infant to “see” and “hear” copies of their own actions.  When speaking to infants, adults typically alter the acoustic properties of their speech in a variety of ways compared with how they speak to other adults; for example, they use higher pitch, increased pitch range that is making the voice go up and down in tone, more pitch variability, and slower speech rate. Recent research by Tanya Broesch and Gregory Bryant has shown that these vocal changes happen similarly, not only across industrialized populations but also in traditional societies.<ref>Broesch TL, Bryant GA. [https://www.researchgate.net/publication/271671965_Prosody_in_Infant-Directed_Speech_Is_Similar_Across_Western_and_Traditional_Cultures Prosody in infant-directed speech is similar across western and traditional cultures.] Journal of Cognition and Development. 2015 Jan 1;16(1):31-43.</ref>


=== Social interaction and mirroring ===
In the 1-2 month period infants continue to pay special attention to faces.  They also start to recognize familiar faces and will smile at them. Their ability to sustain social interaction increases and they are figuring out the process of turn taking which is an inherent aspect of a conversation.  Adult social partners will often imitate infants’ facial expressions, lip and jaw movements and vocalisations which act as mirror that allows the infant to “see” and “hear” copies of their own actions.


=== Sitting ===
=== Spontaneous touches ===
[[File:Babysitting.jpg|thumb|Sitting with hip support]]
During this period infants continue to use their hands to gather information about the surfaces they encounter. Contact with a surface is often associated with exploratory movements of the hand across the surface, or repeated flexion and extension of the fingers and grasping.  Independent movements of the fingers are also frequently seen, especially when the infant is socially engaged or is paying attention to an object within reaching distance. <ref>DiMercurio A, Connell JP, Clark M, Corbetta D. [https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02613/full A naturalistic observation of spontaneous touches to the body and environment in the first 2 months of life.] Frontiers in psychology. 2018 Dec 18;9:2613.</ref>
Infants in the 1 to 2 month period will need support in sitting. Trunk support in sitting has the following characteristics:<ref name=":1" />
* Head falls forward
* Brief period of scapular retraction
* Increased flexion of the elbows
* Forearm pronation
* Extension of the wrist
* Flexion of the fingers
* Feet flexed and everted


At around 6 weeks, infants can extend their thoracic spine and head when supported around their waist. When they are closer to 2 months, infants can maintain a semi-erect position with support at their hips. This facilitates their neck and trunk extensors.<ref name=":1" />
=== Perceptual-Motor Development ===
 During the 1-2 month period the infant is awake and alert for longer periods of time,
 
increasingly responds to sounds and sights from the environment and gains more control of
 
movements of the head and limbs
 
 Head control in supine is improving as the infant learns to hold the head steady in the
 
midline as well as turn the head to locate interesting sights and sounds.
 
 When held upright infants can keep the head erect for extended periods of time.
 
 Neck rotation is still associated with extension and lateral flexion.
 
 Spontaneous movements of the lower extremities, along with the pull of gravity serve to
 
stretch the hip and knee flexor muscles, leading to increased range of extension of the
 
extremity joints.
 
 Postural control of the trunk in supine is improving but movement of the extremities still
 
leads to lateral displacement. and sometimes rotation of the pelvis.
 
 Vigorous kicking of the LEs is often associated with abduction of the shoulders and extension
 
of the elbows as the infant works to maintain a steady torso.
 
 Kicking movements still show strong coupling between the hip and knee joints, however the
 
ability to uncouple hip and knee emerges when the infant engages in goal directed actions of
 
the foot, such as reaching to a target with the foot.
 
 Whole body general movements give way to fidgety general movements from around 9
 
weeks.
 
 Infants start to reach successfully towards interesting objects within reach and start to use
 
finger movements to explore objects.
 
 Infants actively explore surfaces with their feet and hands.
 
 Although simultaneous flexion and extension movements of the fingers are still common,
 
independent movements of the fingers become more prominent.
 
 
 
General and fidgety movements
 
General movements continue to be characterized by writhing movements that involve the head,
 
trunk and extremities in the 1-2 month period. However, towards the end of this period fidgety
 
movements (FMs) are increasingly present (Einspieler 2016)
 
Writhing general movements in a healthy full term infant are described as complex and involve the
 
entire body, notably arm, leg, neck, and trunk movements in variable sequences. They wax and
 
wane, varying in intensity, speed, and range of motion, and have a gradual onset and end.
 
Rotations around the limb axes and slight changes in the direction of movement create the
 
impression of fluency and elegance.
 
FMs are small movements of moderate speed with variable acceleration of the neck, trunk, and
 
limbs in all directions. They may appear as early as six weeks after term, but usually occur from
 
around 9 weeks until 16–20 weeks, occasionally even a few weeks longer. They fade out when
 
antigravity and intentional movements start to dominate.
 
The presence and character of fidgety movements are good indicators of the integrity of the infant&amp;#39;s
 
nervous system (Einspieler 2016).
 
Head control and neck movements
 
At the beginning of the 1-2 month period the infant still tends to lie supine with the head turned to
 
one or the other side. Head rotation continues to be associated with some neck extension and
 
lateral rotation.
 
Head turning may be associated with an asymmetrical tonic neck reflex (ATNR) posture, but this is
 
not obligatory.
 
By the end of the 1-2 month period the infant is more inclined to hold the head in the midline and
 
easily turns the head to look at interesting objects and events in the environment.
 
At this age infants tend to lie with the UEs abducted and extended, a position that helps to stabilize
 
the trunk and provide a stable base for head movements and kicking (Bly 1994).
 
The infant is also able to combine neck rotation with extension of the head to look upwards.
 
However, control of the exact position of the head is clearly still developing, as rotation is usually
 
associated with some neck extension and lateral flexion. This combination of movements suggests
 
that the movement is brought about by contraction of the sternocleidomastoid muscles, with limited
 
action in the deep neck stabilizers (Bly 1994).
 
The 1-2 month old infant is able to visually follow an object from the side to the midline, as well as
 
follow an object moved in a downwards direction. The ability to follow an object across the midline
 
is not present yet.
 
Upper extremity actions
 
During periods of relative quiet the one-month-old infant adopts a variety of postures of the upper
 
extremities. Abduction of the shoulders with the upper arms resting on the support surface is often
 
observed – in this position the UEs serve as outriggers and help to stabilize the trunk when moving
 
the head and lower extremities. Interestingly this co-opting of the UEs for a postural function during
 
this period is associated with a decrease in the occurrence of swiping actions towards objects within
 
reach (von Hofsten 1984).
 
Head rotation may be associated with the fencing position (extension of the face elbow with flexion
 
of the skull elbow). However this association decreases over the 1-2 month period, and importantly,
 
is never obligatory.
 
The tendency to abduct the shoulders and use the UEs as outriggers decreases towards the end of
 
this period when infants start to bring their hands into the midline.
 
At the beginning of the 1-2 month period infants produce large range swiping movements of the
 
upper extremities. These swiping movements are associated with elbow extension and extension of
 
the fingers. The hand comes close to the object, but mostly does not make contact.
 
Over the coming weeks the infant gains more control over reaching movements and starts to reach
 
towards objects within easy reach with greater success. The extension of the fingers seen in the one
 
month old infant become less pronounced (Hofsten 1984).
 
The infant brings the hand into the proximity of the toy and then uses small range movements of the
 
shoulder and elbow to explore different ways of touching and grasping the toy.
 
Towards the end of this period (10-12 weeks), as the infant&amp;#39;s ability to steady the head and trunk
 
when moving the UEs becomes more reliable the infant gains more control of reaching towards toys.
 
They are able to bring the hand into contact with the toy and start to use the fingers to explore it.
 
This is the start of the ability to stabilise the position of the hand in space and at the same time use
 
independent finger movements to gather information about the texture, structure and behaviour of
 
objects. Visual attention to the toy and the hand provides further information that starts to link what
 
is felt and seen.
 
Exploratory hand movements
 
During this period infants continue to use their hands to gather information about the surfaces they
 
encounter. Contact with a surface is often associated with exploratory movements of the hand
 
across the surface, or repeated flexion and extension of the fingers.
 
Independent movements of the fingers are also frequently seen, especially when the infant is
 
socially engaged or is paying attention to an object within reaching distance.
 
Postural sway and postural stability
 
Observing an infant during periods of quiet supine lying allows one to observe the postural sway
 
present in the trunk. These exploratory movements allow the postural system to gather the sensory
 
information needed for estimating the position of the body as a whole and exploring the most
 
effective strategies to maintain a stable posture (Dusing 2013).
 
Here you see Will lying quietly at 5 and 10 weeks of age. The upper arms come to rest on the SS and
 
the continuous lateral sway of the trunk provides changing contact between the upper arm and the
 
SS.
 
Will is also exploring the contact between his feet and the SS, and between each other. This
 
exploration is important for developing the infant&amp;#39;s ability to use surfaces to support their actions.
 
Lower extremity movements
 
The 1-2 month old infant engages in periods of relative calm when the feet rest on the SS with
 
varying amounts of flexion of the hips and knees.
 
At 7 weeks Will&amp;#39;s range of hip and knee extension is still restricted by the remaining physiological
 
flexor stiffness.
 
By 10 weeks Will has close to full range of hip and knee extension. Full hip extension is associated
 
with anterior pelvic tilt.
 
The range of ankle plantar flexion has also increased.
 
The 1-2 month old still engages in extended periods of kicking. Movement patterns include
 
repeated single leg kicking with alternate leg kicking and bilateral hip and knee flexion and
 
extension.
 
At this age hip and knee movements are still coupled. The ankles remain in dorsiflexion with
 
intermittent flexion and extension of the toes.
 
The range of movement of hip flexion and knee extension during kicking movements increases over
 
the 1-2 month period, as seen in these pictures of Will at 10 weeks.
 
LE Bridging
 
From time to time one or both feet push down on the SS. Pushing down with one foot is associated
 
with head and trunk extension and lateral weight shift.
 
Foot reaching
 
Although spontaneous kicking actions tend to still show strong intra-limb coupling, research has
 
shown that 2 month old infants can adapt their LE actions to operate a mobile and make contact
 
with a toy that has been suspended in the midline.
 
Pull-t-sit
 
The infant&amp;#39;s response to the pull-to-sit manoeuvre is often used as a test when assessing motor
 
development. It provides a good measure of the infant&amp;#39;s neck muscle strength as well as the
 
development of effective anticipatory postural rsponses.
 
By the end of the 1-2 month period, infants have learned to anticipate being lifted and will
 
participate in the PTS manoeuvre by engaging the neck and trunk flexor muscles, stiffening the UEs
 
and flexing the hips.
 
The head is held in line with the trunk as the shoulders are lifted.
 
Once in the upright position the head is held erect, and the infant is able to lift the face to look at the
 
person who has pulled him into sitting.
 
Sitting
 
Bly (1994) describes the characteristics of sitting at the beginning of the 1-2 month period as follows:
 
 In supported sitting the head falls forwards although the infant does make brief attempts to
 
hold it up.
 
 The infant&amp;#39;s UEs no longer hang loosely at the infant&amp;#39;s side.
 
 During brief periods of lifting the head there is retraction of the scapulae, increased flexion
 
of the elbows, forearm pronation, extension of the wrist and flexion of the finger.
 
 Bly surmises that the retraction of the scapulae may provide synergistic stability for head
 
lifting.
 
 The hips are flexed, abducted and laterally rotated, the knees more flexed than in the
 
neonatal period, and the ankles are strongly dorsiflexed and everted as in the neonatal
 
period.
 
Over the 1-2 month period the infants gain increasing control of the position of the trunk when they
 
sit with support around the hips.
 
They are able to maintain the trunk in a semi-erect position, with the line of gravity falling anterior
 
the flexion-extension axis of the hips, creating a gravity flexion moment at the hips which is
 
counteracted by the hips extensors. The trunk and neck extensors work to maintain some extension
 
in the trunk.
 
The ability to control the position of the trunk is less well developed when the trunk is tilted
 
backwards so that the LOG falls posterior to the flexion/extension axis of the hips,
 
Notice how this rapid backwards movement initiates a response in the trunk and neck flexors, as
 
well as reactions in his upper limbs as the postural system responds to the rapid backwards
 
movement of the head.
 
Response to being picked up and moved
 
By the end of the 1-2 month period infants have started to anticipate being picked up and moved.
 
They are able to hold the head in line with the trunk when supported around the chest, especially
 
when tipped forwards or laterally.
 
Prone
 
Prone
 
Back to sleep advice. In the past many infants were put into prone for sleeping, but more recently
 
because prone sleeping is a risk factor for sudden infant death, parents are advised to let infants
 
sleep on their backs. As a result many infants do not like being placed in prone, quickly start to cry
 
and also quickly learn to roll from prone to supine.
 
At the beginning of the 1-2 month period, infants start to lift the head up off the support surface (SS)
 
for brief periods of time. This is accomplished by the neck and thoracic extensor muscle activity.
 
When in prone infants engage in active kicking, alternating between unilateral and bilateral hip and
 
knee flexion and extension. The ankles remain dorsiflexed, with the range of dorsiflexion (DF)
 
increasing with full hip and knee flexion and decreasing with hip and knee extension.
 
Unilateral kicking movements are associated with lateral flexion of the trunk with a lateral shift in
 
weight bearing. Bilateral hip and knee flexion is associated with anterior pelvic tilt and a cephalad
 
shift of weight onto the upper torso.
 
Rolling from prone to supine
 
Will&amp;#39;s ability to initiate rolling from prone to supine illustrate how by 5 weeks he has learned to
 
exploit his rapidly developing control of movement of the extremities to initiate a sequence of
 
movements directed towards achieving a goal.
 
After a period of kicking, Will decides that he has had enough of being in prone, and initiates rolling
 
to supine by lifting his head and extending the thoracic spine. These movements are associated with
 
a more extended position of the hips and knees bilaterally and a shift of his COM caudally.
 
He rotates his shoulder girdle back on the right, flexes the right hip then &amp;quot;falls&amp;quot; onto his back.
 
I turn Will back into prone, and he immediately rolls back into supine, this time using a slightly
 
different pattern of movement. He pushes down on his right hand, extends his elbow, and extends
 
the thoracic spine. The COM shifts caudally, but there is no associated extension of the hips. Will
 
collapses onto his left side with the hips still in flexion, and then rolls onto his back.
 
By the end of the 1-2 month period, the infant&amp;#39;s ability to lift the head and extend the thoracic spine
 
has improved. This is associated with taking some weight on the hands.
 
Extension of the neck and thoracic spine is now associated with extension and adduction of the hips.
 
The ROM of extension at the hips and knees has increased. Some of the time flexion of the knee is
 
associated with flexion of the hip, but not always. This is the beginning of uncoupling of movements
 
the hip and knee flexion and the beginning of the ability to dissociate movements of the two joints.
 
References and bibliography
 
Adolph, K.E. and Kretch, K.S. (2015) Gibson’s Theory of Perceptual Learning. International
 
Encyclopedia of the Social &amp;amp; Behavioral Sciences, Second Edition
 
Bly L. (1994) Motor Skills Acquisition in the First Year.  
 
Corbetta, D., Dimercurio, A., Wiener, R. F., Connell, J. P., &amp;amp; Clark, M. (2018). How Perception and
 
Action Fosters Exploration and Selection in Infant Skill Acquisition. Advances in Child Development
 
and Behavior (Vol. 55, pp. 1–29). Elsevier Ltd.
 
Cornwell, K. S., Fitzgerald, H. E., &amp;amp; Harris, L. J. (1985). On the state‐dependent nature of infant head
 
orientation. Infant Mental Health Journal, 6(3), 137-144.
 
Duncan K, Goodworth A, Da Costa CSN, Wininger M, Saavedra S. (2017) Parent handling of typical
 
infants varies segmentally across development of postural control. Exp Brain Res. 236(3):645-654.
 
Einspieler, C., Marschik, P. B., &amp;amp; Prechtl, H. F. R. (2008). Human Motor Behavior Prenatal Origin and
 
Early Postnatal Development. Journal of Psychology, 216(3) 148–154.
 
Lee HM, Galloway JC. (20120 Early intensive postural and movement training advances head control
 
in very young infants. Phys Ther. Jul;92(7):935-47.
 
Lima-Alvarez CD, Tudella E, van der Kamp J, Savelsbergh GJ. (2013) Effects of postural manipulations
 
on head movements from birth to 4 months of age. J Mot Behav. 45(3):195-203.
 
Lima-Alvarez CD, Tudella E, van der Kamp J, Savelsbergh GJ. (2014) Early development of head
 
movements between birth and 4 months of age: a longitudinal study. J Mot Behav. 46(6):415-22.
 
Reddy, V., Markova, G., &amp;amp; Wallot, S. (2013). Anticipatory Adjustments to Being Picked Up in
 
Infancy. PLoS ONE, 8(6), <nowiki>https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3688725/</nowiki>
 
von Hofsten C, Rönnqvist L. (1993) The structuring of neonatal arm movements. Child Dev.
 
64(4):1046-57.
 
von Hofsten C, Rosander K. (2018) The Development of Sensorimotor Intelligence in Infants. Adv
 
Child Dev Behav. 2018;55:73-106.
 
Von Hoften, C, Ronquis L. (1994) Neonatal finger and arm movements as determined by a social
 
context.  Early Development and Parenting . 3(2), 81-94
 
Hofsten, C. Von. (1984). Developmental Changes in the Organization of Pre-reaching Movements.
 
Developmental Psychology 20(3), 378–388.


== Resources ==
== Resources ==

Revision as of 14:43, 1 August 2022

Infant development: 1-2 month period[edit | edit source]

This is an exciting period as it the time when infants’ perceptual-motor behaviour shifts from

spontaneous movements to movements that become increasingly intentional as the infant uses

longer periods of being awake and alert to explore and connect with their social and physical

environment.

  • When awake and alert infants alternate between period of activity and periods of quiet
  • Infants gain greater control of their head posture and movements, increasing their ability for

visual reach and information gathering

  • They look towards interesting sounds and visual events in the environment
  • They actively explore their bodies, clothing and surrounding surfaces with their hands and

their feet

  • They are becoming increasingly self-aware and develop an awareness of their own bodies

relative to the surrounding physical environment

  • Reaching becomes more intentional and successful.

Communication and social behaviour[edit | edit source]

During this period infants’ communication and social behaviour increases and become more

complex, sustained and expressive.

  • First smiles occur
  • They turn their heads towards voices and other sounds in the environment
  • Quieten their limb movements or smile in response to sounds
  • Show increasing interest in faces and recognise familiar faces
  • Make eye contact
  • Cry differently for different needs (e.g. hungry vs tired)
  • Produce a range of pre-speech sounds – known as protophones - that include grunts, coos

and gurgles.

Vocalisation[edit | edit source]

During this period infants produce a range of pre-speech sounds, known as protophones.

  • These sounds include grunts, squeals, coos, and vowel like sounds.
  • Vocalizations becomes more extended and include changes in emotional tone (known as inflexion)
  • Vocal behavior is often produced during social interaction but is also heard when infants are not

attending to the environment.

This spontaneous vocalization allows the infant to practice and explore different ways of producing a

range of different sounds which provides the foundation for later babbling and words.

Social interaction and mirroring[edit | edit source]

In the 1-2 month period infants continue to pay special attention to faces. They also start to. recognize familiar faces and will smile at them. Their ability to sustain social interaction increases and they are figuring out the process of turn taking which is an inherent aspect of a conversation. Adult social partners will often imitate infants’ facial expressions, lip and jaw movements and vocalizations which act as mirror that allows the infant to “see” and “hear” copies of their own actions. When speaking to infants, adults typically alter the acoustic properties of their speech in a variety of ways compared with how they speak to other adults; for example, they use higher pitch, increased pitch range that is making the voice go up and down in tone, more pitch variability, and slower speech rate. Recent research by Tanya Broesch and Gregory Bryant has shown that these vocal changes happen similarly, not only across industrialized populations but also in traditional societies.[1]

Social interaction and mirroring[edit | edit source]

In the 1-2 month period infants continue to pay special attention to faces. They also start to recognize familiar faces and will smile at them. Their ability to sustain social interaction increases and they are figuring out the process of turn taking which is an inherent aspect of a conversation. Adult social partners will often imitate infants’ facial expressions, lip and jaw movements and vocalisations which act as mirror that allows the infant to “see” and “hear” copies of their own actions.

Spontaneous touches[edit | edit source]

During this period infants continue to use their hands to gather information about the surfaces they encounter. Contact with a surface is often associated with exploratory movements of the hand across the surface, or repeated flexion and extension of the fingers and grasping. Independent movements of the fingers are also frequently seen, especially when the infant is socially engaged or is paying attention to an object within reaching distance. [2]

Perceptual-Motor Development[edit | edit source]

 During the 1-2 month period the infant is awake and alert for longer periods of time,

increasingly responds to sounds and sights from the environment and gains more control of

movements of the head and limbs

 Head control in supine is improving as the infant learns to hold the head steady in the

midline as well as turn the head to locate interesting sights and sounds.

 When held upright infants can keep the head erect for extended periods of time.

 Neck rotation is still associated with extension and lateral flexion.

 Spontaneous movements of the lower extremities, along with the pull of gravity serve to

stretch the hip and knee flexor muscles, leading to increased range of extension of the

extremity joints.

 Postural control of the trunk in supine is improving but movement of the extremities still

leads to lateral displacement. and sometimes rotation of the pelvis.

 Vigorous kicking of the LEs is often associated with abduction of the shoulders and extension

of the elbows as the infant works to maintain a steady torso.

 Kicking movements still show strong coupling between the hip and knee joints, however the

ability to uncouple hip and knee emerges when the infant engages in goal directed actions of

the foot, such as reaching to a target with the foot.

 Whole body general movements give way to fidgety general movements from around 9

weeks.

 Infants start to reach successfully towards interesting objects within reach and start to use

finger movements to explore objects.

 Infants actively explore surfaces with their feet and hands.

 Although simultaneous flexion and extension movements of the fingers are still common,

independent movements of the fingers become more prominent.


General and fidgety movements

General movements continue to be characterized by writhing movements that involve the head,

trunk and extremities in the 1-2 month period. However, towards the end of this period fidgety

movements (FMs) are increasingly present (Einspieler 2016)

Writhing general movements in a healthy full term infant are described as complex and involve the

entire body, notably arm, leg, neck, and trunk movements in variable sequences. They wax and

wane, varying in intensity, speed, and range of motion, and have a gradual onset and end.

Rotations around the limb axes and slight changes in the direction of movement create the

impression of fluency and elegance.

FMs are small movements of moderate speed with variable acceleration of the neck, trunk, and

limbs in all directions. They may appear as early as six weeks after term, but usually occur from

around 9 weeks until 16–20 weeks, occasionally even a few weeks longer. They fade out when

antigravity and intentional movements start to dominate.

The presence and character of fidgety movements are good indicators of the integrity of the infant&#39;s

nervous system (Einspieler 2016).

Head control and neck movements

At the beginning of the 1-2 month period the infant still tends to lie supine with the head turned to

one or the other side. Head rotation continues to be associated with some neck extension and

lateral rotation.

Head turning may be associated with an asymmetrical tonic neck reflex (ATNR) posture, but this is

not obligatory.

By the end of the 1-2 month period the infant is more inclined to hold the head in the midline and

easily turns the head to look at interesting objects and events in the environment.

At this age infants tend to lie with the UEs abducted and extended, a position that helps to stabilize

the trunk and provide a stable base for head movements and kicking (Bly 1994).

The infant is also able to combine neck rotation with extension of the head to look upwards.

However, control of the exact position of the head is clearly still developing, as rotation is usually

associated with some neck extension and lateral flexion. This combination of movements suggests

that the movement is brought about by contraction of the sternocleidomastoid muscles, with limited

action in the deep neck stabilizers (Bly 1994).

The 1-2 month old infant is able to visually follow an object from the side to the midline, as well as

follow an object moved in a downwards direction. The ability to follow an object across the midline

is not present yet.

Upper extremity actions

During periods of relative quiet the one-month-old infant adopts a variety of postures of the upper

extremities. Abduction of the shoulders with the upper arms resting on the support surface is often

observed – in this position the UEs serve as outriggers and help to stabilize the trunk when moving

the head and lower extremities. Interestingly this co-opting of the UEs for a postural function during

this period is associated with a decrease in the occurrence of swiping actions towards objects within

reach (von Hofsten 1984).

Head rotation may be associated with the fencing position (extension of the face elbow with flexion

of the skull elbow). However this association decreases over the 1-2 month period, and importantly,

is never obligatory.

The tendency to abduct the shoulders and use the UEs as outriggers decreases towards the end of

this period when infants start to bring their hands into the midline.

At the beginning of the 1-2 month period infants produce large range swiping movements of the

upper extremities. These swiping movements are associated with elbow extension and extension of

the fingers. The hand comes close to the object, but mostly does not make contact.

Over the coming weeks the infant gains more control over reaching movements and starts to reach

towards objects within easy reach with greater success. The extension of the fingers seen in the one

month old infant become less pronounced (Hofsten 1984).

The infant brings the hand into the proximity of the toy and then uses small range movements of the

shoulder and elbow to explore different ways of touching and grasping the toy.

Towards the end of this period (10-12 weeks), as the infant&#39;s ability to steady the head and trunk

when moving the UEs becomes more reliable the infant gains more control of reaching towards toys.

They are able to bring the hand into contact with the toy and start to use the fingers to explore it.

This is the start of the ability to stabilise the position of the hand in space and at the same time use

independent finger movements to gather information about the texture, structure and behaviour of

objects. Visual attention to the toy and the hand provides further information that starts to link what

is felt and seen.

Exploratory hand movements

During this period infants continue to use their hands to gather information about the surfaces they

encounter. Contact with a surface is often associated with exploratory movements of the hand

across the surface, or repeated flexion and extension of the fingers.

Independent movements of the fingers are also frequently seen, especially when the infant is

socially engaged or is paying attention to an object within reaching distance.

Postural sway and postural stability

Observing an infant during periods of quiet supine lying allows one to observe the postural sway

present in the trunk. These exploratory movements allow the postural system to gather the sensory

information needed for estimating the position of the body as a whole and exploring the most

effective strategies to maintain a stable posture (Dusing 2013).

Here you see Will lying quietly at 5 and 10 weeks of age. The upper arms come to rest on the SS and

the continuous lateral sway of the trunk provides changing contact between the upper arm and the

SS.

Will is also exploring the contact between his feet and the SS, and between each other. This

exploration is important for developing the infant&#39;s ability to use surfaces to support their actions.

Lower extremity movements

The 1-2 month old infant engages in periods of relative calm when the feet rest on the SS with

varying amounts of flexion of the hips and knees.

At 7 weeks Will&#39;s range of hip and knee extension is still restricted by the remaining physiological

flexor stiffness.

By 10 weeks Will has close to full range of hip and knee extension. Full hip extension is associated

with anterior pelvic tilt.

The range of ankle plantar flexion has also increased.

The 1-2 month old still engages in extended periods of kicking. Movement patterns include

repeated single leg kicking with alternate leg kicking and bilateral hip and knee flexion and

extension.

At this age hip and knee movements are still coupled. The ankles remain in dorsiflexion with

intermittent flexion and extension of the toes.

The range of movement of hip flexion and knee extension during kicking movements increases over

the 1-2 month period, as seen in these pictures of Will at 10 weeks.

LE Bridging

From time to time one or both feet push down on the SS. Pushing down with one foot is associated

with head and trunk extension and lateral weight shift.

Foot reaching

Although spontaneous kicking actions tend to still show strong intra-limb coupling, research has

shown that 2 month old infants can adapt their LE actions to operate a mobile and make contact

with a toy that has been suspended in the midline.

Pull-t-sit

The infant&#39;s response to the pull-to-sit manoeuvre is often used as a test when assessing motor

development. It provides a good measure of the infant&#39;s neck muscle strength as well as the

development of effective anticipatory postural rsponses.

By the end of the 1-2 month period, infants have learned to anticipate being lifted and will

participate in the PTS manoeuvre by engaging the neck and trunk flexor muscles, stiffening the UEs

and flexing the hips.

The head is held in line with the trunk as the shoulders are lifted.

Once in the upright position the head is held erect, and the infant is able to lift the face to look at the

person who has pulled him into sitting.

Sitting

Bly (1994) describes the characteristics of sitting at the beginning of the 1-2 month period as follows:

 In supported sitting the head falls forwards although the infant does make brief attempts to

hold it up.

 The infant&#39;s UEs no longer hang loosely at the infant&#39;s side.

 During brief periods of lifting the head there is retraction of the scapulae, increased flexion

of the elbows, forearm pronation, extension of the wrist and flexion of the finger.

 Bly surmises that the retraction of the scapulae may provide synergistic stability for head

lifting.

 The hips are flexed, abducted and laterally rotated, the knees more flexed than in the

neonatal period, and the ankles are strongly dorsiflexed and everted as in the neonatal

period.

Over the 1-2 month period the infants gain increasing control of the position of the trunk when they

sit with support around the hips.

They are able to maintain the trunk in a semi-erect position, with the line of gravity falling anterior

the flexion-extension axis of the hips, creating a gravity flexion moment at the hips which is

counteracted by the hips extensors. The trunk and neck extensors work to maintain some extension

in the trunk.

The ability to control the position of the trunk is less well developed when the trunk is tilted

backwards so that the LOG falls posterior to the flexion/extension axis of the hips,

Notice how this rapid backwards movement initiates a response in the trunk and neck flexors, as

well as reactions in his upper limbs as the postural system responds to the rapid backwards

movement of the head.

Response to being picked up and moved

By the end of the 1-2 month period infants have started to anticipate being picked up and moved.

They are able to hold the head in line with the trunk when supported around the chest, especially

when tipped forwards or laterally.

Prone

Prone

Back to sleep advice. In the past many infants were put into prone for sleeping, but more recently

because prone sleeping is a risk factor for sudden infant death, parents are advised to let infants

sleep on their backs. As a result many infants do not like being placed in prone, quickly start to cry

and also quickly learn to roll from prone to supine.

At the beginning of the 1-2 month period, infants start to lift the head up off the support surface (SS)

for brief periods of time. This is accomplished by the neck and thoracic extensor muscle activity.

When in prone infants engage in active kicking, alternating between unilateral and bilateral hip and

knee flexion and extension. The ankles remain dorsiflexed, with the range of dorsiflexion (DF)

increasing with full hip and knee flexion and decreasing with hip and knee extension.

Unilateral kicking movements are associated with lateral flexion of the trunk with a lateral shift in

weight bearing. Bilateral hip and knee flexion is associated with anterior pelvic tilt and a cephalad

shift of weight onto the upper torso.

Rolling from prone to supine

Will&#39;s ability to initiate rolling from prone to supine illustrate how by 5 weeks he has learned to

exploit his rapidly developing control of movement of the extremities to initiate a sequence of

movements directed towards achieving a goal.

After a period of kicking, Will decides that he has had enough of being in prone, and initiates rolling

to supine by lifting his head and extending the thoracic spine. These movements are associated with

a more extended position of the hips and knees bilaterally and a shift of his COM caudally.

He rotates his shoulder girdle back on the right, flexes the right hip then &quot;falls&quot; onto his back.

I turn Will back into prone, and he immediately rolls back into supine, this time using a slightly

different pattern of movement. He pushes down on his right hand, extends his elbow, and extends

the thoracic spine. The COM shifts caudally, but there is no associated extension of the hips. Will

collapses onto his left side with the hips still in flexion, and then rolls onto his back.

By the end of the 1-2 month period, the infant&#39;s ability to lift the head and extend the thoracic spine

has improved. This is associated with taking some weight on the hands.

Extension of the neck and thoracic spine is now associated with extension and adduction of the hips.

The ROM of extension at the hips and knees has increased. Some of the time flexion of the knee is

associated with flexion of the hip, but not always. This is the beginning of uncoupling of movements

the hip and knee flexion and the beginning of the ability to dissociate movements of the two joints.

References and bibliography

Adolph, K.E. and Kretch, K.S. (2015) Gibson’s Theory of Perceptual Learning. International

Encyclopedia of the Social &amp; Behavioral Sciences, Second Edition

Bly L. (1994) Motor Skills Acquisition in the First Year.  

Corbetta, D., Dimercurio, A., Wiener, R. F., Connell, J. P., &amp; Clark, M. (2018). How Perception and

Action Fosters Exploration and Selection in Infant Skill Acquisition. Advances in Child Development

and Behavior (Vol. 55, pp. 1–29). Elsevier Ltd.

Cornwell, K. S., Fitzgerald, H. E., &amp; Harris, L. J. (1985). On the state‐dependent nature of infant head

orientation. Infant Mental Health Journal, 6(3), 137-144.

Duncan K, Goodworth A, Da Costa CSN, Wininger M, Saavedra S. (2017) Parent handling of typical

infants varies segmentally across development of postural control. Exp Brain Res. 236(3):645-654.

Einspieler, C., Marschik, P. B., &amp; Prechtl, H. F. R. (2008). Human Motor Behavior Prenatal Origin and

Early Postnatal Development. Journal of Psychology, 216(3) 148–154.

Lee HM, Galloway JC. (20120 Early intensive postural and movement training advances head control

in very young infants. Phys Ther. Jul;92(7):935-47.

Lima-Alvarez CD, Tudella E, van der Kamp J, Savelsbergh GJ. (2013) Effects of postural manipulations

on head movements from birth to 4 months of age. J Mot Behav. 45(3):195-203.

Lima-Alvarez CD, Tudella E, van der Kamp J, Savelsbergh GJ. (2014) Early development of head

movements between birth and 4 months of age: a longitudinal study. J Mot Behav. 46(6):415-22.

Reddy, V., Markova, G., &amp; Wallot, S. (2013). Anticipatory Adjustments to Being Picked Up in

Infancy. PLoS ONE, 8(6), https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3688725/

von Hofsten C, Rönnqvist L. (1993) The structuring of neonatal arm movements. Child Dev.

64(4):1046-57.

von Hofsten C, Rosander K. (2018) The Development of Sensorimotor Intelligence in Infants. Adv

Child Dev Behav. 2018;55:73-106.

Von Hoften, C, Ronquis L. (1994) Neonatal finger and arm movements as determined by a social

context.  Early Development and Parenting . 3(2), 81-94

Hofsten, C. Von. (1984). Developmental Changes in the Organization of Pre-reaching Movements.

Developmental Psychology 20(3), 378–388.

Resources[edit | edit source]

References[edit | edit source]

  1. Broesch TL, Bryant GA. Prosody in infant-directed speech is similar across western and traditional cultures. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2015 Jan 1;16(1):31-43.
  2. DiMercurio A, Connell JP, Clark M, Corbetta D. A naturalistic observation of spontaneous touches to the body and environment in the first 2 months of life. Frontiers in psychology. 2018 Dec 18;9:2613.