Motor Learning Principles to Alter Gait Deviations: Difference between revisions

No edit summary
No edit summary
Line 44: Line 44:
# '''External focus of attention''' is directed towards the effect of the movement on the environment.   
# '''External focus of attention''' is directed towards the effect of the movement on the environment.   
# '''Internal focus of attention''' is directed towards components of the body's movement  
# '''Internal focus of attention''' is directed towards components of the body's movement  
</blockquote>A 15-year review of literature found that research on focus of attention has consistently shown that an external focus enhances motor performance and learning when compared to internal focus.<ref>Wulf G. [http://gwulf.faculty.unlv.edu/wp-content/uploads/2018/11/Wulf_AF_review_2013.pdf Attentional focus and motor learning: a review of 15 years]. International Review of sport and Exercise psychology. 2013 Sep 1;6(1):77-104.</ref>  However, there are some conflicting and contradictory opinions regarding this opinion.  Historically, physiotherapists have underutilised the concept of external focus of attention during cueing, prompting, and feedback.<ref name=":1" />   
</blockquote>
 
 
'''ADD VIDEO COMPARING TWO TYPES?'''
 
A 15-year review of literature found that research on focus of attention has consistently shown that an external focus enhances motor performance and learning when compared to internal focus.<ref>Wulf G. [http://gwulf.faculty.unlv.edu/wp-content/uploads/2018/11/Wulf_AF_review_2013.pdf Attentional focus and motor learning: a review of 15 years]. International Review of sport and Exercise psychology. 2013 Sep 1;6(1):77-104.</ref>  However, there are some conflicting and contradictory opinions regarding this opinion.  Historically, physiotherapists have underutilised the concept of external focus of attention during cueing, prompting, and feedback.<ref name=":1" />   


Elmar Kal and colleagues published a series of studies observing interactions between physiotherapists and patients with stroke.  Specifically they assessed how the physiotherapists worded their feedback and cues during therapy interventions.  They found that only 33% of the therapists were using an external focus of attention, and often after a cue with external focus the feedback would often shift back to an internal focus.<ref>Kal E, van den Brink H, Houdijk H, van der Kamp J, Goossens PH, van Bennekom C, Scherder E. [https://bura.brunel.ac.uk/bitstream/2438/20074/2/FullText.pdf How physical therapists instruct patients with stroke: an observational study on attentional focus during gait rehabilitation after stroke]. Disability and rehabilitation. 2018 May 8;40(10):1154-65.</ref>  For rehabilitation practice, an individualised use of attentional focus instructions may be more effective than an exclusive reliance on external focus instructions, especially if a patients’ motor, sensory, and attentional functioning may be impaired.<ref>Kal E, Houdijk H, van der Kamp J, Verhoef M, Prosée R, Groet E, Winters M, van Bennekom C, Scherder E. [https://vbn.aau.dk/ws/files/334793016/FullText.pdf Are the effects of internal focus instructions different from external focus instructions given during balance training in stroke patients? A double-blind randomized controlled trial.] Clinical rehabilitation. 2019 Feb;33(2):207-21.</ref>  When used appropriately, both external and internal focus of attention can improve performance and learning.<ref name=":1" />   
Elmar Kal and colleagues published a series of studies observing interactions between physiotherapists and patients with stroke.  Specifically they assessed how the physiotherapists worded their feedback and cues during therapy interventions.  They found that only 33% of the therapists were using an external focus of attention, and often after a cue with external focus the feedback would often shift back to an internal focus.<ref>Kal E, van den Brink H, Houdijk H, van der Kamp J, Goossens PH, van Bennekom C, Scherder E. [https://bura.brunel.ac.uk/bitstream/2438/20074/2/FullText.pdf How physical therapists instruct patients with stroke: an observational study on attentional focus during gait rehabilitation after stroke]. Disability and rehabilitation. 2018 May 8;40(10):1154-65.</ref>  For rehabilitation practice, an individualised use of attentional focus instructions may be more effective than an exclusive reliance on external focus instructions, especially if a patients’ motor, sensory, and attentional functioning may be impaired.<ref>Kal E, Houdijk H, van der Kamp J, Verhoef M, Prosée R, Groet E, Winters M, van Bennekom C, Scherder E. [https://vbn.aau.dk/ws/files/334793016/FullText.pdf Are the effects of internal focus instructions different from external focus instructions given during balance training in stroke patients? A double-blind randomized controlled trial.] Clinical rehabilitation. 2019 Feb;33(2):207-21.</ref>  When used appropriately, both external and internal focus of attention can improve performance and learning.<ref name=":1" />   
Line 55: Line 60:


=== Sensory Preference ===
=== Sensory Preference ===
Next, I want to talk on the concept of sensory preference and learning optimal movement. Sensory preference is another theory of importance that I think we can apply to altering gait. We do not all learn the same way, we tend to have a preferred sensory learning style or system. There are three basic sensory systems that we use to learn to walk or move different. You have visual, auditory, and kinesthetic. So we talk about some people profess to learn better by seeing it, some people need to hear it, and some people need to feel it, and some people do well with a combination.  
'''ADD IMAGE OF THREE CIRCLES FROM VIDEO''' <blockquote>'''Three sensory systems involved in mobility:'''<ref name=":1" />
 
# Visual
# Auditory
# Kinesthetic
</blockquote>So we talk about some people profess to learn better by seeing it, some people need to hear it, and some people need to feel it, and some people do well with a combination.  


So how do we determine, as a clinician, whether a patient has a preferred sensory system to use when we're cueing, prompting, and providing feedback. Well, you can ask them, do you learn better by seeing, hearing, or feeling? What I've used over the years is this question of, "What three adjectives would you use to describe a day at the beach?" And if their adjectives are "It's warm, the sand is between my toes, I feel the breeze on my ear," they tend to be a kinaesthetic learner. If they say like, "I hear the waves crashing, the kids are squealing on the beach, and the seagulls are calling out," they're going to be, tend to be an auditory learner. And if they say, "The sky is blue, the sand is brown, I see the waves rolling in," those kinds of adjectives suggest they're a visual learner. I've used this question quite a lot, and it's interesting. I've had some patients give me adjectives for all three, and my observation is those individuals tend to be professional artists or a dedicated recreational artist. But if we can identify that we can fine tune our communication with our clients.  
So how do we determine, as a clinician, whether a patient has a preferred sensory system to use when we're cueing, prompting, and providing feedback. Well, you can ask them, do you learn better by seeing, hearing, or feeling? What I've used over the years is this question of, "What three adjectives would you use to describe a day at the beach?" And if their adjectives are "It's warm, the sand is between my toes, I feel the breeze on my ear," they tend to be a kinaesthetic learner. If they say like, "I hear the waves crashing, the kids are squealing on the beach, and the seagulls are calling out," they're going to be, tend to be an auditory learner. And if they say, "The sky is blue, the sand is brown, I see the waves rolling in," those kinds of adjectives suggest they're a visual learner. I've used this question quite a lot, and it's interesting. I've had some patients give me adjectives for all three, and my observation is those individuals tend to be professional artists or a dedicated recreational artist. But if we can identify that we can fine tune our communication with our clients.  

Revision as of 17:06, 16 July 2022

Original Editor - Stacy Schiurring based on the course by Damien Howell

Top Contributors - Stacy Schiurring, Kim Jackson and Jess Bell

Introduction[edit | edit source]

The development of gait deviations can be a normal part of the aging process. Research has shown that approximately 85% of persons aged 60 years ambulate with "normal gait," however that number decreases to 20% by age 85.[1]. A 2018 systematic review by Herssens et al looked at development of gait deviations across the lifespan. They found as people age, they tend to develop a "cautious gait pattern" which is characterized by (1) decreased walking speed, (2) decreased cadence, (3) decreased step or stride length, and (4) increased step time. [2] Gait deviations can also have a developmental, musculoskeletal, or neurological etiology.[1]

From a rehabilitation perspective, reasons to alter an individual's gait deviation include:

  1. To improve pain
  2. To improve endurance
  3. To improve efficiency
  4. To improve performance
  5. To improve quality of movements
  6. To prevent injury
  7. To improve safety
  8. To decrease need for assistive devices

Motor Learning[edit | edit source]

Motor learning differs from symptom modification. A symptom modification procedure can quickly but temporarily alter an individual's performance, often within a single treatment session.[3]

Motor learning can be defined as: "a change in the capability of a person to perform a skill that must be inferred from a relatively permanent improvement in performance as a result of practice or experience."[4] Performance tends to improve as an individual practices a new skill. While these improvements can occur quickly they also tend to be temporary in nature. True learning occurs when practice continues repeatedly. This allows the new skill to be sustained over time. To learn more about motor learning and motor learning theories, please read this article.

The most effective way to progress from performance to learned behaviour is to use scientific principles and theories of motor learning. However, translating theory to clinical practice provides an opportunity for creativity on the part of the rehabilitation professional. The use of language, cues, feedback, and or prompts are particularly powerful tools.[3]

ADD VIDEO ABOUT THEORIES?

Putting Theory into Practice[edit | edit source]

When adapting motor learning theory into clinical practice, it can be helpful to use a framework to shape how learning will occur. Like any rehabilitation plan of care, this framework will be individualised to best meet the needs of the patient, ie the learner. The rehabilitation professional, ie the teacher, can gain insight into the learner's needs during the patient interview and evaluation, and continuously reassess for needed changes or adjustments as therapy progresses.

Considerations for both teacher and learner:[3]

  1. Focus of Attention: internal vs external
  2. Sensory Preference
  3. What Analogy works best
  4. Progress from explicit to implicit learning
  5. Exploratory learning
  6. Whole vs Part – Segments Sequencing
  7. Mindset attitude
  8. Autonomy
  9. Cueing, prompting, feedback

Focus of Attention[edit | edit source]

Two types of focus of attention:

  1. External focus of attention is directed towards the effect of the movement on the environment.
  2. Internal focus of attention is directed towards components of the body's movement


ADD VIDEO COMPARING TWO TYPES?

A 15-year review of literature found that research on focus of attention has consistently shown that an external focus enhances motor performance and learning when compared to internal focus.[5] However, there are some conflicting and contradictory opinions regarding this opinion. Historically, physiotherapists have underutilised the concept of external focus of attention during cueing, prompting, and feedback.[3]

Elmar Kal and colleagues published a series of studies observing interactions between physiotherapists and patients with stroke. Specifically they assessed how the physiotherapists worded their feedback and cues during therapy interventions. They found that only 33% of the therapists were using an external focus of attention, and often after a cue with external focus the feedback would often shift back to an internal focus.[6] For rehabilitation practice, an individualised use of attentional focus instructions may be more effective than an exclusive reliance on external focus instructions, especially if a patients’ motor, sensory, and attentional functioning may be impaired.[7] When used appropriately, both external and internal focus of attention can improve performance and learning.[3]

Alessandro Piccoli et al 2018 looked at focus on attention in patients with neurological central nervous system (CNS) deficits as compared to those with musculoskeletal disorders. When comparing patients with stroke or idiopathic Parkinsons Disease (PD) to healthy subjects, there was conflicting evidence about the use of the optimal attentional strategy. External focus of attention did not always improve motor learnings compared to internal focus of attention due to the pathological motor impairments of stroke and PD. Changes in sensory pathways including vision and proprioception were also found to effect attentional strategy use. This study found that external focus of attention is more effective than internal focus of attention in affecting motor learning in patients with musculoskeletal disorders.[8]

There is research that supports that novice learners are likely to do better with an internal focus of attention, whereas experts do better with external focus of attention. There is evidence that younger learners likely do better with an external focus, whereas older learners can do well with external and or internal.[3]

Rebecca Gose et al 2021 have suggested that the dichotomy between external focus of attention and internal focus of attention functions as a spectrum. They suggest that seeing focus of attention as a binary paradigm overlooks the dynamic and constantly changing interactions between the individual, the task, and the environment.[9] Whether the chosen focus of attention is internal or external, it is an important clinical consideration to use words that are consistent with the patient's preferred sensory learning system.[3]

Sensory Preference[edit | edit source]

ADD IMAGE OF THREE CIRCLES FROM VIDEO

Three sensory systems involved in mobility:[3]

  1. Visual
  2. Auditory
  3. Kinesthetic

So we talk about some people profess to learn better by seeing it, some people need to hear it, and some people need to feel it, and some people do well with a combination.

So how do we determine, as a clinician, whether a patient has a preferred sensory system to use when we're cueing, prompting, and providing feedback. Well, you can ask them, do you learn better by seeing, hearing, or feeling? What I've used over the years is this question of, "What three adjectives would you use to describe a day at the beach?" And if their adjectives are "It's warm, the sand is between my toes, I feel the breeze on my ear," they tend to be a kinaesthetic learner. If they say like, "I hear the waves crashing, the kids are squealing on the beach, and the seagulls are calling out," they're going to be, tend to be an auditory learner. And if they say, "The sky is blue, the sand is brown, I see the waves rolling in," those kinds of adjectives suggest they're a visual learner. I've used this question quite a lot, and it's interesting. I've had some patients give me adjectives for all three, and my observation is those individuals tend to be professional artists or a dedicated recreational artist. But if we can identify that we can fine tune our communication with our clients.

So we're all familiar with the term audio-visual, where you hear and see, so that's what we're doing right here. I'm going to suggest that in terms of learning to move or alter gait, we can have kinesio-visual, or a person that tends to be kinesio-audio, or the artist who is audio-visual-kinesio preference.

What Analogy works best[edit | edit source]

Next, I want to talk about the concept of using analogies to teach and to learn to move better. We in physical therapy have used analogies for quite some time, the common one is out there was Robin McKenzie who used the analogy of a jelly donut for the lumbar disc or cervical disc oozing out backwards and if you can do extension exercises, you can decrease sciatica and cause centralisation. That's an analogy, and it's a very effective way of communicating to the patient. So analogy identifies a target image and connects that new information to old information. The best image can connect to something that's familiar to the learner. It's best if they are consistent with the client's sensory perception, and I'll give some examples of how we can apply auditory sensory to gait later. It's preferable to have the image external to the body.

I forgot to mention in terms of preferred sensory systems, estimates are that a majority of the population, their preferred sensory system is to see it, to see an image. Maybe a third of the population in whole prefer to hear it and a smaller percentage of the population prefer to feel it. So majority of the population is going to want an analogy that provides an image or a vision, something that they can see. It's preferable if we use an analogy that is short, so that it can be into a mantra that the client can use when they're walking or running.

So I'm going to provide some examples of what I think are walking or running analogies that I've used to communicate with a patient when they're trying to alter their walking and running to move in a more optimal way. Knee separation is to walking with a diaper, keeps daylight between the knees. A stable pelvis, imagine your pelvis is a bucket full of water, don't let it spill out the side. Toes straight is to the alignment of a car, the front tires on a car. Kneecap leg alignment, keep the kneecap pointing down that road like a headlight. Don't let the headlight veer off to either side. A good step length is stepping on each railroad tile for somebody that's too long a step or too short a step. Give them that image. Walk as if you're walking in deep snow or in shallow water to get hip flexion and knee flexion. Walking across a small narrow bridge is going to help an individual that's got a base of support that's too wide or waddling. Punch the foot back towards the ground to get heel off the ground and to propel up and forward. Quiet walking is to sneaking up on somebody, an animal stalking. Those are some analogies that can be used for communication.

Can I progress from explicit to implicit[edit | edit source]

I want to touch briefly on implicit learning versus explicit learning. Explicit learning is at a conscious level whereas implicit is unconscious. When we learn to walk, we didn't go through gait training. It was automatic. That still can occur. Implicit learning tends to be more durable. It sticks if we can do that. Some experts argue that if you have an analogy, it's thought to be more implicit, more automatic, you can kind of keep it in the back of your mind. Again, this dichotomy of implicit learning versus explicit learning is probably more of a spectrum. Both occur, but we need to be aware that if we can make the practice, the reinforcement of the new gait pattern more at an implicit level, it's going to stick better.

So some examples of implicit learning to alter a person's gait would be barefoot running. If you've been a shod runner, and then you switched to running without shoes, you're going to change your step length, your cadence, your footstrike automatically. Backwards walking. I started using that with my status post ACL patients or status post total knee when they didn't have, when they kept that habit of having a stiff knee during swing phase. And I said, hold on, walk backwards. And when they walk backwards, I point out to them that they then bend their knee in swing phase, because they got 135 degrees of flexion, but they tend not to use any of it because of habit. Backwards walking has been used for cerebral palsy patients, Parkinson's patients, and hemiplegic patients. Split treadmill or constraint therapy is a form of implicit learning. Robotic gait training is implicit learning. I think showing my runner on a YouTube video what an Olympic 10,000-metre runner looks like gives them some implicit learning that that's positive, that's what I'm going to do. Melding, I call it melding.

Exploratory learning[edit | edit source]

Next. I want to talk about compare and contrast or exploratory learning. This is a young lady, 70 years young, a runner and has a chronic left gluteal tendinopathy. And this is her preferred or natural running gait. And my assessment was there's limited daylight between the knees, the left foot tends to cross the midline. So my symptom modification procedure, see something, do something. I showed it to the client and I said, can you now run and have some daylight between your knees? So this is the immediate result within the session. And I think you'll see that now there's more daylight and less of the left foot crossing the midline. So I showed her the deviation, I said, "See that, can you do something different?" When she came back, I queried the patient, the client, what does that feel like? What do you hear? What do you see? Right? And sometimes it requires some prompting, more often, the first comment by the patient is, "It feels weird." And we're going to talk about the side effects of the interventions.

So the next step is I now have the patient run the new way a couple of laps, and then I say, "Run the old way, a couple laps, but I want you to finish with the new way. And then I want you to practice that." It's okay when you go do your walking that you do it the old way sometimes because feeling the contrast, you're going to begin to appreciate the new way may have less pain or no pain. Exploratory learning, contrast.

Whole vs Part – Segments Sequencing[edit | edit source]

Considerations in terms of sequencing and segmenting whole versus part. Sometimes you need to break it down into parts. Sometimes you can have them practice the whole thing. So how do I decide when to break it down into parts? Well, first off, if it's a spatiotemporal deviation, it's likely better to let them practise the whole thing. If it's not a spatiotemporal component, I tend to break it down into parts and use the dance step to nowhere. The part's better when I break it down into parts or segments it allows me to use this concept of a preferred sensory, so I can do it slow enough and they can listen to it or feel it or begin to see it. I also use the parts method when there's disparate body regions. When I see a trunk rotation is too great and foot crossing the midline that's top and bottom, I break it down into parts so that we can talk about it and the patient can learn.

So this is an example, this young lady is status post right partial knee replacement. Her preferred gait pattern is, despite the joint replacement, is to have excessive out toeing. So she's doing the dance step to nowhere, the right leg is in stance phase. That's status post knee surgery. And I say, I point out to her that she was toed out. I say, toe in, now dance this way back and forth, back and forth. And you can do the compare and contrast. Finish with the new way. I break it down into parts, focusing on stance leg. The next step is when you do the dance step and you got the stance phase correct, is walk this way. Walk away, and then you can do the compare and contrast. Well, while they're walking, let them shift from the old way to the new way and back and forth, breaking it down into parts.

Mindset attitude[edit | edit source]

Next, I want to talk about choosing the mindset of a positive language versus negative language, the prevailing belief that's, I think developing, is positive is better than negative. Working on strength is probably better than weakness. Both work, both working on weakness and both working on strengths work. But if we can speak in a positive way, engage placebo and minimise nocebic effects it's better. So see that, do that, as opposed to see that don't do that. Hear that, it's quiet. Hear that clomp, don't do that. Feel that, feel that buttock muscle engage to control the pelvis, do that. Feel that it's not engaged, don't do that. Positive would be the new way, negative would be the old way. Positive would be walk with less pain, no pain, negative would be with pain.

Gabriele Wulf et al., the authors of the OPTIMAL theory of motor learning, basically the group profess the importance of external focus of attention, speak to this issue of enhanced expectancies is the best practice to learn, to move better. What's an enhanced expectancy? It's a range of forward-directed anticipatory predictive cognitions or beliefs about what is expected to occur. So, if we provide knowledge of performance and knowledge of results with positive information, it can enhance the learner's expectations that they will have positive results in a similar task in the future. When we provide normative feedback, it suggests that we, kind of, tell a white lie, you're doing better than average. You're doing better than most people. That's going to improve their motivation, improve their self-efficacy, commenting on a peer group performance, such as, "Other people with similar injuries tend to do well when they work on this skill, this can help to increase the patient's or client's expectations that they also will do well. Then the other thing that I think we need to consider is setting easier liberal goals or sub-goals or thresholds or bandwidth. Well, the goals should be challenging, we need to be adjusting them so that the patient is having success.

Autonomy[edit | edit source]

Next, I want to talk about autonomy and self-choice. It's considered very important to achieve best practice when learning to move in an optimal way. We need to give the learner control of the conditions to enhance their learning, need to give them a choice of their instructional language, sensory preference, positive negative. Give them a choice of practice conditions. Some interesting research on learning to throw darts at a target. Had two groups, one group, they allowed individuals to choose the colour of the darts and the other group they didn't give them a choice of colour. When they were just given a choice of the colour of which darts they were going to throw, they performed better and learned better. It's a funny phenomenon.

So self-control enhances motivation. Interventions need to be designed for the individual characteristics and adjusted based on the individual's response. One size does not fit all. In the reference list for this module, providing an example of a template or a process of taking a specific gait deviation. In this module, we're going to talk about contralateral pelvic drop and list internal focus of attention, cues, prompts, feedback. And for each, I've identified a visual, auditory, and kinaesthetic, and then the template uses external focus of attention, and again, provides vocabulary for visual, auditory, and kinaesthetic. And then we're going to apply that in future modules for other common gait deviations.

Who provides cueing, prompting, feedback[edit | edit source]

So good cueing, prompting, and feedback. Start with an external focus of attention and try to include an image or an analogy, try to make it in positive placebo language and then adjust it according to the individual's preferred learning sensory system, and then provide them with a choice. So the first time you can start with external focus and then let them practise it and then give them a choice of internal and let them choose between internal and external focus of attention. We need to eventually make those cues and prompts and feedback short, need to try to make it so that they can use contrast and explore on their own and recognise that we want to try to get it to an implicit level.

Resources[edit | edit source]

References[edit | edit source]

  1. 1.0 1.1 Ataullah AHM, De Jesus O. Gait Disturbances. [Updated 2021 Feb 7]. In: StatPearls [Internet]. Treasure Island (FL): StatPearls Publishing; 2021 Jan Available:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK560610/#article-22020.r6 (accessed 12/07/2022)
  2. Herssens N, Verbecque E, Hallemans A, Vereeck L, Van Rompaey V, Saeys W. Do spatiotemporal parameters and gait variability differ across the lifespan of healthy adults? A systematic review. Gait & posture. 2018 Jul 1;64:181-90.
  3. 3.0 3.1 3.2 3.3 3.4 3.5 3.6 3.7 Howell, D. Gait Analysis. Motor Learning Principles to Alter Gait Deviations. Physioplus. 2022.
  4. Angin S, Simsek I, editors. Comparative kinesiology of the human body: normal and pathological conditions. Academic Press; 2020 Mar 17. p453-466.
  5. Wulf G. Attentional focus and motor learning: a review of 15 years. International Review of sport and Exercise psychology. 2013 Sep 1;6(1):77-104.
  6. Kal E, van den Brink H, Houdijk H, van der Kamp J, Goossens PH, van Bennekom C, Scherder E. How physical therapists instruct patients with stroke: an observational study on attentional focus during gait rehabilitation after stroke. Disability and rehabilitation. 2018 May 8;40(10):1154-65.
  7. Kal E, Houdijk H, van der Kamp J, Verhoef M, Prosée R, Groet E, Winters M, van Bennekom C, Scherder E. Are the effects of internal focus instructions different from external focus instructions given during balance training in stroke patients? A double-blind randomized controlled trial. Clinical rehabilitation. 2019 Feb;33(2):207-21.
  8. Piccoli A, Rossettini G, Cecchetto S, Viceconti A, Ristori D, Turolla A, Maselli F, Testa M. Effect of attentional focus instructions on motor learning and performance of patients with central nervous system and musculoskeletal disorders: A systematic review. Journal of Functional Morphology and Kinesiology. 2018 Jul 25;3(3):40.
  9. Gose R, Abraham A. Looking beyond the binary: an extended paradigm for focus of attention in human motor performance. Experimental Brain Research. 2021 Jun;239(6):1687-99.