Newborn Perceptual Motor Behaviour

Original Editor - Robin Tacchetti based on the course by Pam Versfeld

Top Contributors - Robin Tacchetti, Robin Leigh Tacchetti, Tarina van der Stockt, Ewa Jaraczewska, Kim Jackson and Jess Bell  

Introduction[edit | edit source]

When newborn infants (0-4 weeks old) are awake and alert and lying on a firm surface they respond to visual and auditory events in the environment and actively engage in spontaneous movements of the limbs. The looking, listening and moving provides opportunities for linking what they do to what is seen, heard and felt, creating the perception-action loops that are the basis for making the shift from spontaneous exploratory movements to intentional, goal directed actions that allow the infant to interact with people, things and events in their environment.[1]

Limb Movement Synergies at Birth[edit | edit source]

Newborn perceptual motor synergies.jpg

The multi-segmented structure of the body provides the basis for producing the varied movement patterns seen in human actions. To simplify control of the many degrees of freedom inherent in a multi-segmented body, spontaneous infant movements are constrained and organised into synergies.[2]

  • Lower limb synergy pattern includes intralimb coupling of
    • hip flexion, knee flexion and dorsiflexion
    • hip extension and knee extension
  • Upper limb synergy pattern includes a combination of
    • shoulder and elbow extension with extension of the fingers and wrist;
    • flexion of the elbow with finger flexion.[3]

These whole body movements, called general movements or GMs,  are complex and involve the entire body, notably arm, leg, neck, and trunk movements in  variable sequences.[3]

Over the first few months as the infant explores different ways of interacting with the environment and as the frontal motor areas become more active, the strong intralimb coupling lessens as movement are adapted to allow for effective interaction with the environment. [1]

Head Posture[edit | edit source]

  • Head turned to one side
    In supine the newborn's head is mostly rotated to one side, often with a preference for a particular side, usually to the right.[4] The reason for this tendency is unclear.
  • A prominent feature of head rotation in the first two months is the tendency for rotation to be coupled with neck extension and lateral flexion to the opposite side, which is a reflection of the balance in activity between the sternocleidomastoid (SCM) muscles and deep neck flexor muscle activity.[5]
  • Head in midline.jpg
    Over the first few weeks the infant learns to turn the head to the midline, and can sustain the position briefly, especially when supported by visual attention to an interesting person, object or event.[1]
  • Typically the head is held in the mid-position for brief periods of time when the infant is actively moving the limbs or is distressed.[6]
  • Over the next few weeks the infant will develop the the bilateral antigravity neck muscle strength and control needed to counteract the force of gravity (which creates a turning moment acting on the Center of Gravity (COG) of the head) and maintain the head in midline for longer periods of time.
Partial rolling.jpg

Newborn Rolling[edit | edit source]

In neonates with typical/normal limb stiffness (muscle tone), head turning may initiate partial rolling to the side. This response may be due to the neonatal neck righting reflex[5] but may also be because turning of the neck shifts the infants weight laterally which destabilises the trunk and the infant "'topples" over to side lying.

Visual Attention[edit | edit source]

  • From the first weeks infants pay attention to interesting objects that come into their field of vision. Visual attention is usually associated with cessation of limb movements.
  • When the head is supported in the midline the newborn infant will look at the face of a caregiver for extended periods of time. Infants will turn the head away when they need a break from the intensity of this focussed social interaction.
  • When the head is supported the newborn can move the head to bring the social partner's face into the centre of the visual field.
  • When paying attention to a social partner, the infant will mirror facial expressions and even stick out the tongue if this has been demonstrated.
  • Newborn infants engage in sustained visual regard of their own hands. Interestingly, they also pay close attention to the hands of a caregiver. [3]

Resources[edit | edit source]

References[edit | edit source]

  1. 1.0 1.1 1.2 Versfeld, P. SfA Infant Perceptual Motor Development
  2. Von Hofsten C. Mastering reaching and grasping: The development of manual skills in infancy. InAdvances in psychology 1989 Jan 1 (Vol. 61, pp. 223-258). North-Holland.
  3. 3.0 3.1 3.2 Versfeld, P. Newborn Perceptual Motor Behaviour Course. Physioplus. 2021
  4. Rönnqvist L, Hopkins B. (1998) Head position preference in the human newborn: a new look. Child Dev. 69(1):13-23.
  5. 5.0 5.1 Bly L, Ariz TN. Motor skills acquisition in the first year, an illustrated guide to normal development.
  6. Cornwell, K. S., Fitzgerald, H. E., & Harris, L. J. (1985). On the state‐dependent nature of infant head orientation. Infant Mental Health Journal, 6(3), 137-144.