Hand Function 7-24 Month Period: Difference between revisions

No edit summary
(changed entire page per instructor request)
Line 3: Line 3:


== Introduction ==
== Introduction ==
Infants transitioning to toddlerhood start using their hands in more complex ways. In early infancy, hand use is subjected to random spontaneous movements of the arms. But as they age, infants engage in intentional movement patterns to reach for and grasp objects that they seek.<ref name=":2">Versfeld, P.  Hand Function 7-24 Months Course. Physioplus. 2022</ref> Successful reaching requires an infant to locate the target while opening and aligning the hand in preparation to grasp the object. Developmentally, reaching precedes grasping.<ref name=":1">Chinn LK, Noonan CF, Hoffmann M, Lockman JJ. [https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00009/full Development of infant reaching strategies to tactile targets on the face]. Frontiers in psychology. 2019:9.</ref> Between 9 and 19 months old, infants can plan ahead before reaching a target.<ref>Kaur M, Detherage A, Needham AW. [https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/S0885201420300356 Unconventional tool use in infants: Using a familiar tool in a novel way in the second year of life]. Cognitive Development. 2020 Apr 1;54:100881.</ref> During this period, reaching and grasping are not limited to visual targets as infants can reach for auditory targets as well.<ref name=":1" /> Throughout the 7 to 24 month period, infants will use their hands in a variety of ways. They also use different finger grasps to learn about, manipulate, and play with objects.<ref name=":2" />
Hand function from 7-24 months can be divided into two periods.  


== Development of Hand Function ==
During 7-12 month period infants continue to use their hands to explore the properties of objects, but are also learning to adapt their grasp to the shape and size of objects, and learn to reach for, grasp and move objects in many different ways.  In addition they start to use their hands for communication.
There are two important ideas to keep in mind when considering the development of hand function: cascades in development and perception-action loops with embedded exploration and selection.<ref name=":2" />


* Cascades in development: 
During 12 – 24 month period infants start to use their hands to explore the many different ways that objects can be grasped, moved and manipulated relative to one another to perform many tasks and goal directed actions. <ref name=":0">Versfeld, P.  Hand Function 7-24 months Course.  Physioplus. 2022</ref>
** Development can be viewed as a cascade, with new behaviours building on established ones
** Thus, new skills are a combination of established development and new skill acquisition


* Perception-action loops with embedded exploration and selection:
=== The importance of experience in the development of hand function ===
** Infants will use repeated trials to learn new skills
When considering the development of hand function there are 2 important ideas that need to be kept in mind: cascades in development and perception-action loops with embedded exploration and selection. <ref name=":0" />
** With each opportunity, they learn how to vary their movement to attain success
*** The infant receives different sensory feedback as a consequence of their movement<ref name=":2" />


=== Perception ===
==== Cascades in development ====
<blockquote>"Perception is the process by which we gather information about the physical structure of, and events in, the environment. Perception makes sense of the continuous flow of sensory information received by the brain by recognising patterns within the flow of sensory signals."<ref name=":2" /></blockquote>It is an active process where the brain recognises patterns from the steady flow of sensory information. Examples of the perception of sensory information include tasting, feeling, looking and listening. For infants, hand use provides opportunities to learn about the properties of objects through touch.<ref name=":2" />
Development can be viewed as a cascade, with new behaviors building on established ones. At any moment, the whole child is a product of all the previous developments, and any new change begins with and must build on those previous developments. <ref>Smith LB. It’s all connected: [https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3858855/ Pathways in visual object recognition and early noun learning.] American Psychologist. 2013 Nov;68(8):618.</ref>
== 7 to 12 Month Period ==
Hands serve several purposes during the 7 to 12 month period:<ref name=":2" />
[[File:R 9m sitting between blocks .jpg|thumb|253x253px|using hand for support]]
# They are part of the postural and locomotor systems
#* Infants learn to: balance in sitting; crawl; buttock shuffle; cruise
# They are used for communication and in social interactions
# They are used to pick up, move and manipulate objects (i.e. to explore an object's different properties and find out what can be done with them)<ref name=":2" />


=== Hand-Object Actions ===
The development of hand function clearly illustrates this principle: newborn spontaneous movements of the arms and hands are replaced by intentional use of the hands to explore surfaces and objects in ever more complex ways which allows the infant to start finding out how the hands can be used to pick up, move and manipulate objects to perform tasks to achieve a variety of goals.  
During the 7 to 12 month time period, infants will use their hands to interact with objects using various complex actions. Hand-object behaviours include:<ref name=":2" />


* Shaking
==== Perception-action loops with embedded exploration and selection ====
* Banging
Infants will repeat a new task many times allowing for repeated opportunities to use information about the success of their actions to vary their movements, and over time select and reproduce what works.
* Patting, poking, stroking, flapping with the fingers
 
* Turning objects over
Performing an action over and over again creates perception-action loops, with each repetition producing a slightly different sequence of movements and consequently slightly different sensory feedback. <ref name=":1">Corbetta D, DiMercurio A, Wiener RF, Connell JP, Clark M. [https://www.researchgate.net/publication/326518409_How_Perception_and_Action_Fosters_Exploration_and_Selection_in_Infant_Skill_Acquisition How perception and action fosters exploration and selection in infant skill acquisition. Advances in child development and behavior.] 2018 Jan 1;55:1-29.</ref>
* Passing objects from one hand to the other
 
* Throwing and dropping objects
Some variations in the pattern of movement will be more effective in achieving the goal, and over time will be selected when the task is performed again.
 
Corbetta et al. (2018) calls this process of repetition and adaptation perception-action loops with embedded exploration and selection. It is argued that the embedded exploration and selection process provide the fundamental mechanism by which the discovery and formation of new behaviors occur. <ref name=":1" />
 
=== During the 7-12 period infants use their hands for 3 different functions ===
 
# As part of the postural and locomotor systems including learning to balance in sitting, crawling and buttock shuffling, cruising.  
# For communication and in social interactions
# For picking up, moving and manipulating objects to explore their different properties and what can be done with them.
 
=== Hand behaviors seen in the 7-12 month period. ===
During the 7-12 month period infants continue to explore the properties of objects using a wide range of different and increasingly complex hand-object actions.  
 
These include:
 
* Shaking – especially toys that make a noise
* Banging – especially toys and surfaces that are hard.
* Patting, poking, stroking, flapping with the fingers  
* Turning objects  over many times to inspect them from all angles  
* Passing objects  from one hand to the other
* Throwing and dropping objects becomes more common
* Picking up, moving and dropping small and large objects
* Picking up, moving and dropping small and large objects
<nowiki>**</nowiki> By 12 months, an infant's reach and grasp motions are similar to that of an adult<ref>Sacrey LA, Zwaigenbaum L, Bryson S, Brian J, Smith IM. T[https://link.springer.com/article/10.1186/s11689-018-9259-4 he reach-to-grasp movement in infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder: a high-risk sibling cohort study]. Journal of neurodevelopmental disorders. 2018 Dec;10(1):1-1.</ref>
* Mouthing objects
 
These hand-object interactions provide many opportunities for infants to learn to select a grasp  pattern that is suited to the action that is being performed.
 
Experience with handling and manipulating many different objects is important for learning to adapt the pattern of finger movement to suite the shape and size of different objects. This is the exploration phase and with repetition the most effective pattern is selected when encountering the same object at a later time.
 
Grasp  patterns commonly seen during this period:
 
* Hook grasp
* Power grasp
* Palmar grasp,
* Circular grasp
* Chuck grasp
* Key grasp    
* Pincer grasp<ref>Skills For Action:  The Different Ways We Use our Hands to Grasp, Hold, Move and Manipulate Objects:  Available from: https://skillsforaction.com/different-ways-we-use-our-hands-everyday-function</ref>          
 
=== Postural stability and hand use ===
At the beginning of the 7-12 month period when infants are still learning to sit independently, their head and trunk stability and balance are still insecure and arm actions are needed to maintain balance. <ref>Harbourne RT, Lobo MA, Karst GM, Galloway JC. Sit happens: [https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/S0163638313000441 Does sitting development perturb reaching development, or vice versa?]. Infant Behavior and Development. 2013 Jun 1;36(3):438-50.</ref><ref>Kyvelidou A, Stuberg WA, Harbourne RT, Deffeyes JE, Blanke D, Stergiou N. [https://www.nature.com/articles/pr2009107 Development of upper body coordination during sitting in typically developing infants]. Pediatric research. 2009 May;65(5):553-8.</ref>
 
This means that some of the brain’s limited attention resources are required to balance, and hand actions, especially reaching beyond arm’s length are somewhat restricted.
 
However, the provision of external support will overcome this constraint and allow the infant to explore ways to interact with toys and other objects and to start reaching beyond arm’s length.                                                               
 
==== Detailed inspection and exploration of a toy ====
During 7-12 month period, when infants first encounter a new toy they will often systematically engage in a series of exploratory actions that provide them with information about the object
 
Actions include repeatedly shaking and banging the toy, turning a toy over, fingering it, mouthing and passing it from one hand to the other.
 
These varied actions provide the infant’s perceptual-motor brain with a detailed internal map of the toy – its size, shape, texture and the way the different parts move relative to one another.  
 
These exploratory actions provide experience in using a variety of grasp patterns to accommodate the shape and size of the toy.                    
 
These many exploratory actions infants engage in provide them with detailed information about the shape, size, weight and texture of objects as well as how different properties of objects allow for different actions – known as affordances.
 
So for instance balls are good for throwing, small balls can be thrown with one hand, but larger balls require a two handed grasp. Containers such as tubs and tins afford different actions and are good for putting toys into and can be tipped upside down to get the toys out again.
 
==== Ball activities provide a wide variety of experience ====
Playing with large and small balls provides infants with a wide variety of experience and knowledge about how ball behave. Importantly when a ball is thrown it lands and then rolls across the floor until something or someone stops its forwards motion.
 
Infants and toddlers are drawn to the movement of the ball and learn to visually track the moving ball as well as predict where it will be at a future time.  Predicting where a ball will be at a future time is important for learning the catch a ball.  
 
==== Picking up small objects ====
Around about 10-11 months infants start to take an interest in picking up very small objects.
 
And will also use the extended forefinger to poke at small objects they encounter.
 
The first attempts to pick up small objects usually involves bending and extending the fingers using a raking action.
 
Over time infants start to use a key or pincer grasp for picking up small objects.  
 
In a key grasp the object is held between the thumb pad and the side of the forefinger.
 
In a precision grip the object is held between the pads of the thumb and the forefinger.  This requires more thumb opposition.
 
==== Social interaction and communication gestures ====
By the end of the 7-12 month period infants have learned a range of social interaction and communication gestures.  These include making requests, visual and arm pointing, waving, imitating actions of social partners and the start of pretend play.
 
They will often have learned to clap their hands, raise arms for hooray, and give a high five.
 
By 12 months infants have also started to play pretend games and quickly learn to pretend to talk on a mobile phone and pretend feeding a doll.  
 
=== Autism Navigator Resources ===
A wonderful source of evidence informed information about the development of social and  communication skills.
 
https://babynavigator.com/resources/
 
https://babynavigator.com/wp-content/uploads/2021/04/Milestones-that-Matter-Most-from-1-24-Months.pdf
 
== The 12-24 month period ==
During this period infants learn to use visual attention, motor planning and reach, grasp, hold, transport and manipulation abilities to perform a range of goal directed actions.  
 
=== Here are some of the hand tasks toddlers master in the second year ===
 
* Stacking blocks, building towers
 
* Fitting objects through apertures – posting objects
 
* Fitting and taking off lids
 
* Pulling apart and pushing together
 
* Throwing and intercepting (catching) balls
 
* Retrieving objects from a container and tipping objects out of a container.
 
* Lifting, moving and carrying large or heavy objects
 
* Using a spoon for feeding self
 
* Finger feeding
 
* Drinking from a cup
 
       
 
    
 
=== Task structure ===
Learning to perform more complex goal oriented tasks requires an understanding of the structure of the task and how to combine several different hand-object–surface interactions to complete a task.
 
The structure of a task refers to the series of the actions needed to complete the task including:
 
* Visual information gathering needed for planning movements
 
* Anticipatory and compensatory postural responses  in response to limb movements
* The sequence of movements needed to complete the task
 
=== Fitting lids onto bottles ===
There are several steps in fitting a lid onto a bottle
 
1 Turn the lid upside down and then grasp it around its edge
 
2 Position the lid over the mouth of the bottle
 
3 Fit the lid onto the bottle and then push it down or twist it to complete the action.    
 
Fitting lids and taking them off is usually a bimanual action, with one hand holding the bottle and the other hand manipulating the lid.
 
       
 
   
 
=== Posting activities ===
Inserting or posting objects through small openings or into small spaces is another activity that toddlers enjoy doing. In homes with mobile toddlers this interest often leads to lost keys and other objects that toddlers’ value highly such as cell (mobile) phones and car keys.
 
Toddlers also take great pleasure in activities that involve posting small balls, rods and flat disks into holes in the lids of containers. In these activities the size and shape of the holes and objects being posted can be varied to suit the abilities of the toddler.


=== Grasp Patterns ===
==== The structure of posting  a disk through a slot task ====
When infants learn to sit independently, they become more efficient at reaching and grasping as their arms are no longer needed for support. Their grasp transitions from a reflexive palmar grasp to a radial palmar grasp to a pincer grasp. These fine motor adaptations facilitate more detailed manipulation of, and play with, objects. Using new grasps, infants begin releasing objects awkwardly at around 7 to 8 months. This becomes more purposeful as they age. By around 12 months, infants use fingering and hand-to-hand manipulations to investigate objects.<ref name=":0">Gee BM, Engle J, Parker C, Stanger M, Call H, Peterson TW. [https://journals.healio.com/doi/pdf/10.3928/24761222-20190813-03 Frequency and duration of developmental fine motor patterns in infants and toddlers: a pilot cohort study]. Annals of International Occupational Therapy. 2020 Jan 1;3(1):21-8.</ref> Repetitive trials allow them to find a finger pattern that is best suited to the size and shape of different objects.


Grasp patterns commonly seen during this period include:<ref name=":2" /><ref>[https://skillsforaction.com/different-ways-we-use-our-hands-everyday-function Skills for Action. Accessible at: https://skillsforaction.com/different-ways-we-use-our-hands-everyday-function]</ref>
# Visual search locates the position of the disk.
# The disk is picked up using a grip that aligns the disk vertically.
# Visual attention is shifted to the slot in the posting box
# The hand is moved to bring the disk vertically above the slot and aligned with its angle
# The position and alignment of the disk is adapted if necessary.  
# The disc is inserted


* Hook grasp: used to grasp small objects; fingers flexed at all joints
 
* Power grasp: used to hold a cylindrical object between fingers and thumb
* Palmar grasp: used to hold objects or tools with a long handle
* Circular grasp: used to grasp a ball or flat round object
* Chuck grasp: using the thumb, forefinger and ring finger; complex grasp typically only mastered when children are aged 4 or 5 years old
* Key grasp: used to grasp small flat surface between thumb and side of forefinger, other fingers flexed to provide support
* Pincer grasp: a precision grip - infants from around 8 to 10 months of age start to pick up small objects between the tip of their thumb and forefinger


=== Small Objects ===
==== Toddlers are very busy people ====
At around 10 to 11 months old, infants begin to poke and pick up small objects. Poking involves using an extended forefinger while picking involves bending and extending the fingers using a raking action. As time progresses, infants will transition to a pincer or key grip to pick up small objects.<ref name=":2" />
Toddlers are very busy people and in a safe and nurturing environment will often move quickly between different parts of the physical environment, spending short periods of time interacting with interesting objects and toys that they encounter or engage in play that mimics caregiver activities.  


=== Ball Activities ===
In the kitchen for instance drawers and cabinet doors are opened and closed, and contents unpacked. Pots and pans may be used for pretend cooking activities for a brief period of time, before the toddler moves off to find something else to play with.
[[File:Babyball(1).jpg|alt=|right|frameless|300x300px]]
Ball play in infancy creates a variety of experiences to help infants learn how balls function. Infants learn to visually track moving balls as well as predict where the ball will stop moving. Anticipating the future location of a moving ball is an important skill for learning to catch a ball. Ball play with a partner allows infants to observe their partner's actions, discover different ways balls move, and how to catch and throw.<ref name=":2" />


=== Social Interaction and Communication Gestures ===
In natural setting such as their homes and day care setting infants will move around the environment, engaging with  many objects for brief periods of time.
During the 7 to 12 month period, infants use their hands for social interaction and communication. Some of the gestures you might see during this stage are listed below:


* Waving
Corit Herzberg and colleagues from New York University (NYU) conducted a study using frame-by-frame video analyses of spontaneous activity of forty  13-23 month old infants in two 2 hour  home visits. Regardless of age, for every infant and time scale, across 10,015 object bouts, object interactions were short (median = 9.8 s) and varied, with transitions among dozens of toys and non-toys, with this activity consuming an average to 60% of the infants' time.<ref name=":2">Herzberg O, Fletcher KK, Schatz JL, Adolph KE, Tamis‐LeMonda CS. [https://srcd.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cdev.13669 Infant exuberant object play at home: Immense amounts of time‐distributed, variable practice.] Child development. 2022 Jan;93(1):150-64.</ref>  
* Arm pointing
* Clapping hands
* Raising hands for hooray
* High five
* Copying actions<ref name=":2" />


== 12-24 Months ==
The authors of the NYU study suggest that this exuberant object play, creates immense amounts of brief, time-distributed, variable interactions with objects and may optimally promote learning about object properties and functions, motor skill acquisition, and growth in cognitive, social, and language domains.<ref name=":2" />
=== Task Structure ===
During the 12 to 24 month stage, toddlers use their hands for more goal-directed actions.<ref name=":2" /> Their improved visual perceptual and spatial relation skills contribute to their improved ability to manipulate objects and use both hands at once.<ref name=":0" /> Goal-oriented tasks require an understanding of the series of actions needed to complete the task (this is otherwise known as task structure). The three main components needed to complete a task structure are listed below:<ref name=":2" />


# Visual information gathered for planning movements
An important insight provided by this study is that infants naturally learn about their environment using a strategy of brief exploratory interactions with many play and non-play objects. This flitting from one activity to another is not a sign of poor attention span, but rather an indication of employing their time in the most effective way to learn about the properties and affordances of many objects. <ref name=":2" />
# Predict and adapt posture in response to limb movements
# Complete a sequence of movements


Toddlers learn to perform tasks with greater accuracy over time and through repeated experiences. During play activities, toddlers will adapt their grip, their body position and the pulling force applied through their arms to accommodate for the specific task. They quickly learn how to plan, execute and adapt their movements to attain their goal.<ref name=":2" />
.


=== Posting Activities ===
== Resources: ==
[[File:Toddler 22m post disk 2.jpg|right|frameless|250x250px]]
Posting or inserting activities begin when infants are aged between 12 and 24 months. This task requires the toddler to place objects through small openings such as small balls, rods or flat discs inserted into holes in the lids of a container. Through this undertaking, toddlers learn about sizes and shapes of objects as they fit through the various holes.<ref name=":2" />


=== Duration of Play ===
* [https://www.nyuactionlab.com/publications#:~:text=2019%3A,%2C%2063%2C%20180%2D201 Infant Action Lab]  
Toddlers aged between 12 and 24 months have brief sustained periods of focused attention on a task before moving onto another activity. These quick bursts of focused play are not a sign of a poor attention span, but rather they are an indication of toddlers employing their time in the most effective way to learn about the properties and affordances of many objects. "Affordance is defined as what the environment offers the individual and what it provides or furnishes."<ref>Sando OJ, Sandseter EBH. [https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0272494419307078 Affordances for physical activity and well-being in the ECEC outdoor environment]. J. Environ. Psychol. 2020;69:101430. </ref> A study by Herzberg et al.<ref>Herzberg O, Fletcher KK, Schatz JL, Adolph KE, Tamis‐LeMonda CS. [https://drive.google.com/file/d/1yqa1WT-GMXGdDvM4Sii72_mpLIQFUlwL/view Infant exuberant object play at home: Immense amounts of time‐distributed, variable practice]. Child development. 2022 Jan;93(1):150-64.</ref> showed that toddlers' object interactions were on average 9.8 seconds with transitions among dozens of toys and non-toys.


== Resources ==
* [http://tomt.skillsforaction.com/node/699 Skills for Action: Perception-action loops are the basic structure underlying infant learning] 


* [http://www.skillsforaction.com/different-ways-we-use-our-hands-everyday-function Skills For Action]
* [[Hand Function 0-7 months|Hand Function 0-7 Months]]
* [[Hand Function]]
* [[Infant development|Infant Development]]


== References ==
[[Category:Course Pages]]
[[Category:Course Pages]]
[[Category:Rehabilitation]]
[[Category:Rehabilitation]]
[[Category:ReLAB-HS Course Page]]
[[Category:ReLAB-HS Course Page]]
[[Category:Paediatrics]]
[[Category:Paediatrics]]

Revision as of 20:05, 31 July 2022

Introduction[edit | edit source]

Hand function from 7-24 months can be divided into two periods.

During 7-12 month period infants continue to use their hands to explore the properties of objects, but are also learning to adapt their grasp to the shape and size of objects, and learn to reach for, grasp and move objects in many different ways.  In addition they start to use their hands for communication.

During 12 – 24 month period infants start to use their hands to explore the many different ways that objects can be grasped, moved and manipulated relative to one another to perform many tasks and goal directed actions. [1]

The importance of experience in the development of hand function[edit | edit source]

When considering the development of hand function there are 2 important ideas that need to be kept in mind: cascades in development and perception-action loops with embedded exploration and selection. [1]

Cascades in development[edit | edit source]

Development can be viewed as a cascade, with new behaviors building on established ones. At any moment, the whole child is a product of all the previous developments, and any new change begins with and must build on those previous developments. [2]

The development of hand function clearly illustrates this principle: newborn spontaneous movements of the arms and hands are replaced by intentional use of the hands to explore surfaces and objects in ever more complex ways which allows the infant to start finding out how the hands can be used to pick up, move and manipulate objects to perform tasks to achieve a variety of goals.  

Perception-action loops with embedded exploration and selection[edit | edit source]

Infants will repeat a new task many times allowing for repeated opportunities to use information about the success of their actions to vary their movements, and over time select and reproduce what works.

Performing an action over and over again creates perception-action loops, with each repetition producing a slightly different sequence of movements and consequently slightly different sensory feedback. [3]

Some variations in the pattern of movement will be more effective in achieving the goal, and over time will be selected when the task is performed again.

Corbetta et al. (2018) calls this process of repetition and adaptation perception-action loops with embedded exploration and selection. It is argued that the embedded exploration and selection process provide the fundamental mechanism by which the discovery and formation of new behaviors occur. [3]

During the 7-12 period infants use their hands for 3 different functions[edit | edit source]

  1. As part of the postural and locomotor systems including learning to balance in sitting, crawling and buttock shuffling, cruising.  
  2. For communication and in social interactions
  3. For picking up, moving and manipulating objects to explore their different properties and what can be done with them.

Hand behaviors seen in the 7-12 month period.[edit | edit source]

During the 7-12 month period infants continue to explore the properties of objects using a wide range of different and increasingly complex hand-object actions.  

These include:

  • Shaking – especially toys that make a noise
  • Banging – especially toys and surfaces that are hard.
  • Patting, poking, stroking, flapping with the fingers
  • Turning objects  over many times to inspect them from all angles  
  • Passing objects  from one hand to the other
  • Throwing and dropping objects becomes more common
  • Picking up, moving and dropping small and large objects
  • Mouthing objects

These hand-object interactions provide many opportunities for infants to learn to select a grasp  pattern that is suited to the action that is being performed.

Experience with handling and manipulating many different objects is important for learning to adapt the pattern of finger movement to suite the shape and size of different objects. This is the exploration phase and with repetition the most effective pattern is selected when encountering the same object at a later time.

Grasp  patterns commonly seen during this period:

  • Hook grasp
  • Power grasp
  • Palmar grasp,
  • Circular grasp
  • Chuck grasp
  • Key grasp    
  • Pincer grasp[4]          

Postural stability and hand use[edit | edit source]

At the beginning of the 7-12 month period when infants are still learning to sit independently, their head and trunk stability and balance are still insecure and arm actions are needed to maintain balance. [5][6]

This means that some of the brain’s limited attention resources are required to balance, and hand actions, especially reaching beyond arm’s length are somewhat restricted.

However, the provision of external support will overcome this constraint and allow the infant to explore ways to interact with toys and other objects and to start reaching beyond arm’s length.                                                               

Detailed inspection and exploration of a toy[edit | edit source]

During 7-12 month period, when infants first encounter a new toy they will often systematically engage in a series of exploratory actions that provide them with information about the object

Actions include repeatedly shaking and banging the toy, turning a toy over, fingering it, mouthing and passing it from one hand to the other.

These varied actions provide the infant’s perceptual-motor brain with a detailed internal map of the toy – its size, shape, texture and the way the different parts move relative to one another.  

These exploratory actions provide experience in using a variety of grasp patterns to accommodate the shape and size of the toy.                  

These many exploratory actions infants engage in provide them with detailed information about the shape, size, weight and texture of objects as well as how different properties of objects allow for different actions – known as affordances.

So for instance balls are good for throwing, small balls can be thrown with one hand, but larger balls require a two handed grasp. Containers such as tubs and tins afford different actions and are good for putting toys into and can be tipped upside down to get the toys out again.

Ball activities provide a wide variety of experience[edit | edit source]

Playing with large and small balls provides infants with a wide variety of experience and knowledge about how ball behave. Importantly when a ball is thrown it lands and then rolls across the floor until something or someone stops its forwards motion.

Infants and toddlers are drawn to the movement of the ball and learn to visually track the moving ball as well as predict where it will be at a future time.  Predicting where a ball will be at a future time is important for learning the catch a ball.  

Picking up small objects[edit | edit source]

Around about 10-11 months infants start to take an interest in picking up very small objects.

And will also use the extended forefinger to poke at small objects they encounter.

The first attempts to pick up small objects usually involves bending and extending the fingers using a raking action.

Over time infants start to use a key or pincer grasp for picking up small objects.  

In a key grasp the object is held between the thumb pad and the side of the forefinger.

In a precision grip the object is held between the pads of the thumb and the forefinger.  This requires more thumb opposition.

Social interaction and communication gestures[edit | edit source]

By the end of the 7-12 month period infants have learned a range of social interaction and communication gestures.  These include making requests, visual and arm pointing, waving, imitating actions of social partners and the start of pretend play.

They will often have learned to clap their hands, raise arms for hooray, and give a high five.

By 12 months infants have also started to play pretend games and quickly learn to pretend to talk on a mobile phone and pretend feeding a doll.  

Autism Navigator Resources[edit | edit source]

A wonderful source of evidence informed information about the development of social and  communication skills.

https://babynavigator.com/resources/

https://babynavigator.com/wp-content/uploads/2021/04/Milestones-that-Matter-Most-from-1-24-Months.pdf

The 12-24 month period[edit | edit source]

During this period infants learn to use visual attention, motor planning and reach, grasp, hold, transport and manipulation abilities to perform a range of goal directed actions.

Here are some of the hand tasks toddlers master in the second year[edit | edit source]

  • Stacking blocks, building towers
  • Fitting objects through apertures – posting objects
  • Fitting and taking off lids
  • Pulling apart and pushing together
  • Throwing and intercepting (catching) balls
  • Retrieving objects from a container and tipping objects out of a container.
  • Lifting, moving and carrying large or heavy objects
  • Using a spoon for feeding self
  • Finger feeding
  • Drinking from a cup

       

   

Task structure[edit | edit source]

Learning to perform more complex goal oriented tasks requires an understanding of the structure of the task and how to combine several different hand-object–surface interactions to complete a task.

The structure of a task refers to the series of the actions needed to complete the task including:

  • Visual information gathering needed for planning movements
  • Anticipatory and compensatory postural responses  in response to limb movements
  • The sequence of movements needed to complete the task

Fitting lids onto bottles[edit | edit source]

There are several steps in fitting a lid onto a bottle

1 Turn the lid upside down and then grasp it around its edge

2 Position the lid over the mouth of the bottle

3 Fit the lid onto the bottle and then push it down or twist it to complete the action.    

Fitting lids and taking them off is usually a bimanual action, with one hand holding the bottle and the other hand manipulating the lid.

       

   

Posting activities[edit | edit source]

Inserting or posting objects through small openings or into small spaces is another activity that toddlers enjoy doing. In homes with mobile toddlers this interest often leads to lost keys and other objects that toddlers’ value highly such as cell (mobile) phones and car keys.

Toddlers also take great pleasure in activities that involve posting small balls, rods and flat disks into holes in the lids of containers. In these activities the size and shape of the holes and objects being posted can be varied to suit the abilities of the toddler.

The structure of posting  a disk through a slot task[edit | edit source]

  1. Visual search locates the position of the disk.
  2. The disk is picked up using a grip that aligns the disk vertically.
  3. Visual attention is shifted to the slot in the posting box
  4. The hand is moved to bring the disk vertically above the slot and aligned with its angle
  5. The position and alignment of the disk is adapted if necessary.
  6. The disc is inserted

 

Toddlers are very busy people[edit | edit source]

Toddlers are very busy people and in a safe and nurturing environment will often move quickly between different parts of the physical environment, spending short periods of time interacting with interesting objects and toys that they encounter or engage in play that mimics caregiver activities.

In the kitchen for instance drawers and cabinet doors are opened and closed, and contents unpacked. Pots and pans may be used for pretend cooking activities for a brief period of time, before the toddler moves off to find something else to play with.

In natural setting such as their homes and day care setting infants will move around the environment, engaging with  many objects for brief periods of time.

Corit Herzberg and colleagues from New York University (NYU) conducted a study using frame-by-frame video analyses of spontaneous activity of forty  13-23 month old infants in two 2 hour  home visits. Regardless of age, for every infant and time scale, across 10,015 object bouts, object interactions were short (median = 9.8 s) and varied, with transitions among dozens of toys and non-toys, with this activity consuming an average to 60% of the infants' time.[7]

The authors of the NYU study suggest that this exuberant object play, creates immense amounts of brief, time-distributed, variable interactions with objects and may optimally promote learning about object properties and functions, motor skill acquisition, and growth in cognitive, social, and language domains.[7]

An important insight provided by this study is that infants naturally learn about their environment using a strategy of brief exploratory interactions with many play and non-play objects. This flitting from one activity to another is not a sign of poor attention span, but rather an indication of employing their time in the most effective way to learn about the properties and affordances of many objects. [7]

.

Resources:[edit | edit source]

  1. 1.0 1.1 Versfeld, P. Hand Function 7-24 months Course. Physioplus. 2022
  2. Smith LB. It’s all connected: Pathways in visual object recognition and early noun learning. American Psychologist. 2013 Nov;68(8):618.
  3. 3.0 3.1 Corbetta D, DiMercurio A, Wiener RF, Connell JP, Clark M. How perception and action fosters exploration and selection in infant skill acquisition. Advances in child development and behavior. 2018 Jan 1;55:1-29.
  4. Skills For Action: The Different Ways We Use our Hands to Grasp, Hold, Move and Manipulate Objects: Available from: https://skillsforaction.com/different-ways-we-use-our-hands-everyday-function
  5. Harbourne RT, Lobo MA, Karst GM, Galloway JC. Sit happens: Does sitting development perturb reaching development, or vice versa?. Infant Behavior and Development. 2013 Jun 1;36(3):438-50.
  6. Kyvelidou A, Stuberg WA, Harbourne RT, Deffeyes JE, Blanke D, Stergiou N. Development of upper body coordination during sitting in typically developing infants. Pediatric research. 2009 May;65(5):553-8.
  7. 7.0 7.1 7.2 Herzberg O, Fletcher KK, Schatz JL, Adolph KE, Tamis‐LeMonda CS. Infant exuberant object play at home: Immense amounts of time‐distributed, variable practice. Child development. 2022 Jan;93(1):150-64.