Development of Sitting: Difference between revisions

No edit summary
No edit summary
Line 3: Line 3:


== Brief review of the biomechanics of sitting ==
== Brief review of the biomechanics of sitting ==
Maintaining an upright sitting posture requires the ability to align the body segments so that the vertical projection of the center of mass (COM), known as the line of gravity (LOG) falls within the base of support. In infants the COM when sitting lies within the chest. When infants sit erect on a flat surface the trunk is tilted forwards slightly so that the LOG runs just anterior to the flexion/extension axis of the hips.   
Maintaining an upright sitting posture requires the ability to align the body segments so that the vertical projection of the centre of mass (COM), known as the line of gravity (LOG) falls within the base of support.   


In infants the COM when sitting lies within the chest.  
In infants, the COM when sitting lies within the chest.  


When infants sit erect on a flat surface the trunk is tilted forwards slightly so that the LOG runs just anterior to the flexion/extension axis of the hips.  
When infants sit erect on a flat surface, their trunk is tilted forwards slightly so that the LOG runs just anterior to the flexion/extension axis of the hips.  


This creates a flexion moment around the hips which is counteracted by the hip extensor muscles. Because the base of support (BOS) extends forwards over the lower limbs, the trunk can be tilted far forwards without losing balance.
This creates a flexion moment around the hips which is counteracted by the hip extensor muscles. Because the base of support (BOS) extends forwards over the lower limbs, the trunk can be tilted far forwards without the infant losing their balance.
[[File:Pamv40.png|none|thumb|400x400px|alt=]]
[[File:Pamv40.png|none|thumb|400x400px|alt=]]






However, in erect sitting as the LOG fall close to the posterior border of the BOS, tilting the trunk backwards moves the COP beyond posterior edge the BOS making it difficult to maintain balance.  
However, in erect sitting, the LOG falls close to the posterior border of the BOS. This tilts the trunk backwards, moves the COP beyond the posterior edge of the BOS, which makes it difficult to maintain balance.  


Infants learn over time to flex the neck and trunk to stop the LOG from moving past the posterior border of the BOS.
Infants learn over time to flex their neck and trunk to stop the LOG from moving past the posterior border of the BOS.
[[File:Pamv41.png|none|thumb|400x400px]]
[[File:Pamv41.png|none|thumb|400x400px]]


'''In sitting the base of support in the side-to-side direction is relatively narrow'''. Any action that shifts weight onto one buttock moves the LOG to the edge of the BOS.  
'''In sitting, the base of support in the side-to-side direction is relatively narrow'''. Any action that shifts weight onto one buttock moves the LOG to the edge of the BOS.  


This is an unstable position but stability can be restored by realigning the position of the shoulder girdle relative to the pelvis or by adapting a change-in-support strategy to increase the BOS in a sideways direction.<ref name=":0">Kyvelidou A, Stuberg WA, Harbourne RT, Deffeyes JE, Blanke D, Stergiou N. [https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2713810/ Development of upper body coordination during sitting in typically developing infants.] Pediatric research. 2009 May;65(5):553-8.</ref>
This is an unstable position, but stability can be restored by: realigning the position of the shoulder girdle relative to the pelvis; or by adapting a change-in-support strategy to increase the BOS in a sideways direction.<ref name=":0">Kyvelidou A, Stuberg WA, Harbourne RT, Deffeyes JE, Blanke D, Stergiou N. [https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2713810/ Development of upper body coordination during sitting in typically developing infants.] Pediatric research. 2009 May;65(5):553-8.</ref>
[[File:Pamv49.png|none|thumb|400x400px]]
[[File:Pamv49.png|none|thumb|400x400px]]
== Learning to sit has two components: ==
== Learning to sit has two components: ==
● Acquiring the strength and control needed to extend the spine and hold the head and trunk upright and steady.<ref name=":2">Pin TW, Butler PB, Cheung HM, Shum SL. [https://bmcpediatr.biomedcentral.com/articles/10.1186/s12887-019-1791-1 Relationship between segmental trunk control and gross motor development in typically developing infants aged from 4 to 12 months: a pilot study]. BMC pediatrics. 2019 Dec;19(1):1-9.</ref> 


Maintaining balance by adapting the alignment of the head and trunk relative the BOS and responding effectively to forces that perturb the head and trunk.<ref name=":0" />
# Acquiring the strength and control needed to extend the spine and hold the head and trunk upright and steady.<ref name=":2">Pin TW, Butler PB, Cheung HM, Shum SL. [https://bmcpediatr.biomedcentral.com/articles/10.1186/s12887-019-1791-1 Relationship between segmental trunk control and gross motor development in typically developing infants aged from 4 to 12 months: a pilot study]. BMC pediatrics. 2019 Dec;19(1):1-9.</ref>
# Maintaining balance by adapting the alignment of the head and trunk relative to the BOS, and responding effectively to forces that perturb the head and trunk.<ref name=":0" />


== Sitting does not just happen – it requires experience ==
== Sitting does not just happen – it requires experience ==
The postural mechanisms that allow the infant to stabilize the head in space and keep the trunk erect and steady are initially learned from repeated experience of being carried upright and sitting with support over many months.  
The postural mechanisms that allow the infant to stabilise their head in space and keep their trunk erect and steady are initially learned from repeated experience of being carried upright and sitting with support over many months.  


In the early months carers provide maximum support for keeping the trunk erect and stable when carrying infants and also when holding them sitting on their laps. <ref name=":1">Duncan K, Goodworth A, Da Costa CS, Wininger M, Saavedra S. [https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6190590/ Parent handling of typical infants varies segmentally across development of postural control.] Experimental brain research. 2018 Mar;236(3):645-54.</ref>  
In the early months, carers provide maximum support for keeping the trunk erect and stable when carrying infants and also when holding them sitting on their laps.<ref name=":1">Duncan K, Goodworth A, Da Costa CS, Wininger M, Saavedra S. [https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6190590/ Parent handling of typical infants varies segmentally across development of postural control.] Experimental brain research. 2018 Mar;236(3):645-54.</ref>  


Sitting facing outwards on a carer’s lap allows the infant to visually explore the environment and attend to interesting things and events and this has an impact on postural set which is influenced by arousal, motivation and curiosity. <ref>Franchak JM. [https://padlab.ucr.edu/publications/2019-Franchak-Infancy.pdf Changing opportunities for learning in everyday life: Infant body position over the first year]. Infancy. 2019 Mar;24(2):187-209.</ref>
Sitting facing outwards on a carer’s lap allows the infant to visually explore the environment and attend to interesting things and events. This has an impact on postural set, which is influenced by arousal, motivation and curiosity.<ref>Franchak JM. [https://padlab.ucr.edu/publications/2019-Franchak-Infancy.pdf Changing opportunities for learning in everyday life: Infant body position over the first year]. Infancy. 2019 Mar;24(2):187-209.</ref>
[[File:Pamv50.png|none|thumb|400x400px]]
[[File:Pamv50.png|none|thumb|400x400px]]
Over time carers decrease the amount of support they provide and expect the infant to work harder to stay erect. <ref name=":1" />Infants also start to use their upper limbs to provide additional support by holding onto to available surfaces.
Over time, carers decrease the amount of support they provide and expect the infant to work harder to stay erect.<ref name=":1" /> Infants also start to use their upper limbs to provide additional support by holding onto available surfaces.
[[File:Pamv52.png|none|thumb|400x400px]]
[[File:Pamv52.png|none|thumb|400x400px]]
== Learning to extend the neck and spine ==
== Learning to extend the neck and spine ==
Over the first 6 months repeated experience of being picked up, carried and supported in sitting provides the experience needed for strengthening the spinal muscles and learning to hold the head erect and extend the spine against gravity.  
Over the first six months, repeated experience of being picked up, carried and supported in sitting provides the experience needed for strengthening the spinal muscles and learning to hold the head erect and extend the spine against gravity.  


Infants first learn to keep the head upright and steady when support is provided at the level of the axilla. Over time they progressively gain control over first the thoracic spine and then the lumbar spine extension.<ref name=":3">Saavedra SL, van Donkelaar P, Woollacott MH. [https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3545022/ Learning about gravity: segmental assessment of upright control as infants develop independent sitting]. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2012 Oct 15;108(8):2215-29.</ref><ref name=":6">Sangkarit N, Siritaratiwat W, Bennett S, Tapanya W. [https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8534436/ Factors Associating with the Segmental Postural Control during Sitting in Moderate-to-Late Preterm Infants via Longitudinal Study]. Children. 2021 Sep 26;8(10):851.</ref><ref name=":2" />
Infants first learn to keep the head upright and steady when support is provided at the level of the axilla. Over time, they progressively gain control over first the thoracic spine and then lumbar spine extension.<ref name=":3">Saavedra SL, van Donkelaar P, Woollacott MH. [https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3545022/ Learning about gravity: segmental assessment of upright control as infants develop independent sitting]. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2012 Oct 15;108(8):2215-29.</ref><ref name=":6">Sangkarit N, Siritaratiwat W, Bennett S, Tapanya W. [https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8534436/ Factors Associating with the Segmental Postural Control during Sitting in Moderate-to-Late Preterm Infants via Longitudinal Study]. Children. 2021 Sep 26;8(10):851.</ref><ref name=":2" />


This progressive development of extension of the spine can be seen when infants are supported in sitting at different levels: axilla, thoracic, pelvis.
This progressive development of extension of the spine can be seen when infants are supported in sitting at different levels: the axilla, thoracic spine and pelvis.
[[File:Pamv45.png|none|thumb|400x400px]]
[[File:Pamv45.png|none|thumb|400x400px]]


In typically developing infants carers adapt the support they provide when lifting and carrying the infant, as well as when the infant sits on the carer’s lap.
In typically developing infants, carers adapt the support they provide when lifting and carrying the infant, as well as when the infant sits on the carer’s lap.


As seen in these pictures a small difference in the level of support can make a big difference to the infant's ability to sustain head and trunk extension.<ref name=":3" />
As seen in these pictures, a small difference in the level of support can make a big difference to the infant's ability to sustain head and trunk extension.<ref name=":3" />
[[File:Pamv46.png|none|thumb|400x400px]]
[[File:Pamv46.png|none|thumb|400x400px]]
== Postural orientation and balance ==
== Postural orientation and balance ==
“Postural control is no longer considered simply a summation of static reflexes but, rather, a complex skill based on the interaction of dynamic sensorimotor processes. Sensory information from somatosensory, vestibular and visual systems is integrated, and the relative weights placed on each of these inputs depend on the goals of the movement task and the environmental context. <ref name=":4">Horak FB. [https://www.cs.cmu.edu/~cga/legs/chamr1.pdf Postural orientation and equilibrium: what do we need to know about neural control of balance to prevent falls?]. Age and ageing. 2006 Sep 1;35(suppl_2):ii7-11.</ref>  
“Postural control is no longer considered simply a summation of static reflexes but, rather, a complex skill based on the interaction of dynamic sensorimotor processes [...] Sensory information from somatosensory, vestibular and visual systems is integrated, and the relative weights placed on each of these inputs depend on the goals of the movement task and the environmental context."<ref name=":4">Horak FB. [https://www.cs.cmu.edu/~cga/legs/chamr1.pdf Postural orientation and equilibrium: what do we need to know about neural control of balance to prevent falls?]. Age and ageing. 2006 Sep 1;35(suppl_2):ii7-11.</ref>  


Balance, also known as postural equilibrium, involves the coordination of movement strategies to stabilize the center of body mass during both self-initiated and externally triggered disturbances of stability.  
Balance, also known as postural equilibrium, involves the coordination of movement strategies to stabilise the centre of body mass during both self-initiated and externally triggered disturbances of stability.  


The specific response strategy selected depends not only on the characteristics of the external postural displacement but also on the individual's expectations, goals and prior experience.<ref name=":4" />  
The specific response strategy selected depends not only on the characteristics of the external postural displacement, but also on the individual's expectations, goals and prior experience.<ref name=":4" />  


== Firstly anticipatory postural adjustments ==
== Firstly anticipatory postural adjustments ==
Anticipatory postural adjustments occur prior to voluntary limb movement, and serve to maintain postural stability by initiating muscle contraction to compensate for destabilizing forces associated with moving a limb.  
Anticipatory postural adjustments (APA) occur prior to voluntary limb movement, and serve to maintain postural stability by initiating muscle contraction to compensate for destabilising forces associated with moving a limb.  


An infants’ experience with lifting, moving and vigorously shaking and banging rattles provide them with repeated opportunities to elicit APAs that stabilize the neck and trunk.
An infant's experience with lifting, moving and vigorously shaking and banging rattles provide them with repeated opportunities to elicit APAs that stabilise the neck and trunk.
[[File:Pamv48.png|none|thumb|400x400px]]
[[File:Pamv48.png|none|thumb|400x400px]]


Another important postural strategy that emerges as infants gain more experience of sitting independently is the shift from a bottom up to a top down balance strategy whereby the infant learns to stabilise the head in space and adjust the alignment of the trunk segments to stay upright.
Another important postural strategy that emerges as infants gain more experience of sitting independently is the shift from a bottom up to a top down balance strategy whereby the infant learns to stabilise their head in space and adjust the alignment of their trunk segments to stay upright.


To do this the alignment of the upper and lower thoracic segments and the pelvis are adjusted to keep the head upright and stable.
To do this, the alignment of the upper and lower thoracic segments and the pelvis are adjusted to keep the head upright and stable.
[[File:Pamv53.png|none|thumb|400x400px]]
[[File:Pamv53.png|none|thumb|400x400px]]


However infants will still topple over sideways with the head and trunk moving as a unit when the lateral displacement of their COM is too far or too rapid.
However, infants will still topple over sideways with the head and trunk moving as a unit when the lateral displacement of their COM is too far or too rapid.
[[File:Pamv60.png|none|thumb|400x400px]]
[[File:Pamv60.png|none|thumb|400x400px]]




With experience of independent sitting infants also learn to use change-in-support strategies, involving grasping or stepping movements of the limbs to increase the BOS.<ref name=":5">Maki BE, McIlroy WE. [https://www.researchgate.net/publication/14068312_The_Role_of_Limb_Movements_in_Maintaining_Upright_Stance_The_Change-in-Support_Strategy The role of limb movements in maintaining upright stance: the “change-in-support” strategy]. Physical therapy. 1997 May 1;77(5):488-507.</ref>


When learning to balance in sitting the most common change–in-support strategy used is reaching out to the side and taking weight on the hand when losing balance to the side. This action increases the BoS in a lateral direction, stops the falling CoM and prevents a fall to the side.<ref>Kyvelidou A, Stuberg WA, Harbourne RT, Deffeyes JE, Blanke D, Stergiou N. [https://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.570.8611&rep=rep1&type=pdf Development of upper body coordination during sitting in typically developing infants.] Pediatric research. 2009 May;65(5):553-8.</ref><ref name=":5" />
With experience of independent sitting, infants also learn to use change-in-support strategies, involving grasping or stepping movements of the limbs to increase the BOS.<ref name=":5">Maki BE, McIlroy WE. [https://www.researchgate.net/publication/14068312_The_Role_of_Limb_Movements_in_Maintaining_Upright_Stance_The_Change-in-Support_Strategy The role of limb movements in maintaining upright stance: the “change-in-support” strategy]. Physical therapy. 1997 May 1;77(5):488-507.</ref>
 
When learning to balance in sitting, the most common change–in-support strategy used is reaching out to the side and taking weight on the hand when losing balance to the side. This action increases the BOS in a lateral direction, stops the falling COM and prevents a fall to the side.<ref>Kyvelidou A, Stuberg WA, Harbourne RT, Deffeyes JE, Blanke D, Stergiou N. [https://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.570.8611&rep=rep1&type=pdf Development of upper body coordination during sitting in typically developing infants.] Pediatric research. 2009 May;65(5):553-8.</ref><ref name=":5" />
[[File:Pamv49.png|thumb|alt=|none|400x400px]]
[[File:Pamv49.png|thumb|alt=|none|400x400px]]


== Importance of visual curiosity and desire to reach for toy ==
== Importance of visual curiosity and desire to reach for toy ==
In very young infants lifting the head and keeping it erect requires sustained effort, especially for an infant with weakness of the neck and trunk extensors.  
In very young infants, lifting the head and keeping it erect requires sustained effort, especially for an infant who has weakness of the neck and trunk extensors.  


However, infants are usually willing to put in the effort needed into holding the head erect if there is something really interesting to look at such as a colorful object or an animated face that invites social interaction.  
However, infants are usually willing to put in the effort needed into holding the head erect if there is something really interesting to look at such as a colorful object or an animated face that invites social interaction.  


From about 4 months typically developing infants take a great interest in reaching for, holding and manipulating toys. These actions require anticipatory and reactive muscle activity to stabilise the head and trunk and are important in providing infants with experience needed for learning to sit independently with good balance. <ref>Bertenthal B, Von Hofsten C. [https://www.academia.edu/es/14702088/Eye_Head_and_Trunk_Control_The_Foundation_for_Manual_Development Eye, head and trunk control: The foundation for manual development.] Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 1998 Mar 4;22(4):515-20.</ref>
From about four months, typically developing infants take a great interest in reaching for, holding and manipulating toys. These actions require anticipatory and reactive muscle activity to stabilise the head and trunk and are important in providing infants with the experience needed for learning to sit independently with good balance.<ref>Bertenthal B, Von Hofsten C. [https://www.academia.edu/es/14702088/Eye_Head_and_Trunk_Control_The_Foundation_for_Manual_Development Eye, head and trunk control: The foundation for manual development.] Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 1998 Mar 4;22(4):515-20.</ref>


== The natural progression of sitting development ==
== The natural progression of sitting development ==
The development of sitting involves increasing control over the neck and trunk segments involved in sitting erect and maintaining balance in the head, upper torso, lower torso and pelvis.  
The development of sitting involves increasing control over the neck and trunk segments that are involved in sitting erect and maintaining balance in the head, upper torso, lower torso and pelvis.  


The development of sitting starts with the infant first learning to hold the head erect and steady when supported around the upper chest in an upright position.<ref name=":6" /><ref name=":2" /><ref>van Iersel PA, la Bastide-van Gemert S, Wu YC, Hadders-Algra M. [https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0378378220306113 Alberta Infant Motor Scale: Cross-cultural analysis of gross motor development in Dutch and Canadian infants and introduction of Dutch norms.] Early Human Development. 2020 Dec 1;151:105239.</ref><ref>Gontijo AP, de Melo Mambrini JV, Mancini MC. [https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/S1413355520311382?via%3Dihub Cross-country validity of the Alberta Infant Motor Scale using a Brazilian sample]. Brazilian Journal of Physical Therapy. 2021 Jul 1;25(4):444-9.</ref>
The development of sitting starts with the infant first learning to hold their head erect and steady when supported around the upper chest in an upright position.<ref name=":6" /><ref name=":2" /><ref>van Iersel PA, la Bastide-van Gemert S, Wu YC, Hadders-Algra M. [https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0378378220306113 Alberta Infant Motor Scale: Cross-cultural analysis of gross motor development in Dutch and Canadian infants and introduction of Dutch norms.] Early Human Development. 2020 Dec 1;151:105239.</ref><ref>Gontijo AP, de Melo Mambrini JV, Mancini MC. [https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/S1413355520311382?via%3Dihub Cross-country validity of the Alberta Infant Motor Scale using a Brazilian sample]. Brazilian Journal of Physical Therapy. 2021 Jul 1;25(4):444-9.</ref>
[[File:Pamv61.png|none|thumb|400x400px]]
[[File:Pamv61.png|none|thumb|400x400px]]
Over time and with practice infants learn to stabilize the head and trunk when supported around the waist, and later with support around the pelvis.
Over time and with practice, infants learn to stabilise their head and trunk when supported around the waist, and later with support around the pelvis.


At this stage typically developing infants are often more interested in reaching for and grabbing an interesting toy than maintaining balance.<ref>Mlincek MM, Roemer EJ, Kraemer C, Iverson JM. Posture Matters: [https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/01942638.2022.2027845 Object Manipulation During the Transition to Arms-Free Sitting in Infants at Elevated vs. Typical Likelihood for Autism Spectrum Disorder.] Physical & Occupational Therapy In Pediatrics. 2022 Jan 9:1-5.</ref>
At this stage, typically developing infants are often more interested in reaching for and grabbing an interesting toy than maintaining balance.<ref>Mlincek MM, Roemer EJ, Kraemer C, Iverson JM. Posture Matters: [https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/01942638.2022.2027845 Object Manipulation During the Transition to Arms-Free Sitting in Infants at Elevated vs. Typical Likelihood for Autism Spectrum Disorder.] Physical & Occupational Therapy In Pediatrics. 2022 Jan 9:1-5.</ref>
[[File:Pmv64.png|none|thumb|400x400px]]
[[File:Pmv64.png|none|thumb|400x400px]]


Line 96: Line 97:
[[File:Pamv65.png|none|thumb|400x400px]]
[[File:Pamv65.png|none|thumb|400x400px]]


Once the infant can stabilize the trunk on the pelvis, and sit briefly without arm or external support, typically developing infants start to pay attention to maintaining their balance as they reach out to connect with people and objects in their environment. <ref>Harbourne RT, Lobo MA, Karst GM, Galloway JC. [https://www.semanticscholar.org/paper/Sit-happens%3A-Does-sitting-development-perturb-or-Harbourne-Lobo/acd1933675da681fc942512c4eea0fd065b7d20a Sit happens: Does sitting development perturb reaching development, or vice versa?.] Infant Behavior and Development. 2013 Jun 1;36(3):438-50.</ref>
Once the infant can stabilise their trunk on their pelvis, and sit briefly without arm or external support, typically developing infants start to pay attention to maintaining their balance as they reach out to connect with people and objects in their environment.<ref>Harbourne RT, Lobo MA, Karst GM, Galloway JC. [https://www.semanticscholar.org/paper/Sit-happens%3A-Does-sitting-development-perturb-or-Harbourne-Lobo/acd1933675da681fc942512c4eea0fd065b7d20a Sit happens: Does sitting development perturb reaching development, or vice versa?.] Infant Behavior and Development. 2013 Jun 1;36(3):438-50.</ref>
[[File:Pamv66.png|none|thumb|400x400px]]
[[File:Pamv66.png|none|thumb|400x400px]]


With experience infants learn to reach in all direction, pick up and move objects, and transition into and out of prone kneeling.
With experience, infants learn to reach in all direction, pick up and move objects, and transition into and out of prone kneeling.
[[File:Pamv67.png|none|thumb|400x400px]]
[[File:Pamv67.png|none|thumb|400x400px]]
== THE ROLE OF POSTURAL SWAY ==
== The role of postural sway ==
Humans and other species are unable to sit or stand perfectly still; their bodies continuously sway. Postural sway (or variability) inevitably emerges from the control of a system with many degrees of freedom. <ref name=":7">Carpenter MG, Murnaghan CD, Inglis JT. [https://www.researchgate.net/publication/46034983_Shifting_the_balance_Evidence_of_an_exploratory_role_of_postural_sway Shifting the balance: evidence of an exploratory role for postural sway]. Neuroscience. 2010 Nov 24;171(1):196-204.</ref>  
Humans and other species are unable to sit or stand perfectly still; their bodies continuously sway. Postural sway (or variability) inevitably emerges from the control of a system with many degrees of freedom.<ref name=":7">Carpenter MG, Murnaghan CD, Inglis JT. [https://www.researchgate.net/publication/46034983_Shifting_the_balance_Evidence_of_an_exploratory_role_of_postural_sway Shifting the balance: evidence of an exploratory role for postural sway]. Neuroscience. 2010 Nov 24;171(1):196-204.</ref>  


Postural sway has been traditionally viewed as a non-functional byproduct of a noisy motor system, leading to the concept that minimal sway was indicative of a healthy and stable postural system
Postural sway has been traditionally viewed as a non-functional byproduct of a noisy motor system, leading to the concept that minimal sway was indicative of a healthy and stable postural system.


Contrary to these traditional views Mark Carpenter and colleagues posit that postural sway may be used by the CNS as an exploratory mechanism to ensure that continuous dynamic inputs are provided by multiple sensory systems.<ref name=":7" />   
Contrary to these traditional views, Mark Carpenter and colleagues posit that postural sway may be used by the CNS as an exploratory mechanism to ensure that continuous dynamic inputs are provided by multiple sensory systems.<ref name=":7" />   


Postural sway provides a continuous flow of sensory information from different sources (touch, proprioception, vestibular and visual) needed for estimating the position of the body as a whole in an ongoing manner.  
Postural sway provides a continuous flow of sensory information from different sources (touch, proprioception, vestibular and visual) needed for estimating the position of the body as a whole in an ongoing manner.  
Line 112: Line 113:
Besides providing sensory feedback, these ongoing trunk movements in lateral and anterior-posterior directions allow the infant to explore the boundaries of the BOS and how far the COM can be moved without toppling over.  
Besides providing sensory feedback, these ongoing trunk movements in lateral and anterior-posterior directions allow the infant to explore the boundaries of the BOS and how far the COM can be moved without toppling over.  


The extent of postural sway both in the AP and side to side direction decreases with sitting experience. However the complexity of movements of the COP increases.
The extent of postural sway both in the anterior-posterior and side-to-side direction decreases with sitting experience. However, the complexity of movements of the COP increases.


== Postural sway patterns are adapted when performing a concurrent goal directed task. ==
== Postural sway patterns are adapted when performing a concurrent goal directed task. ==
Laura Claxton and colleagues studied the postural sway patterns of independently sitting infants while either holding or not holding a toy. Infants exhibited less postural sway when holding the toy. This reduction in sway allowed infants to look at and stabilize the toy in their hand.<ref>Claxton LJ, Strasser JM, Leung EJ, Ryu JH, O'Brien KM. [https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/24604626/ Sitting infants alter the magnitude and structure of postural sway when performing a manual goal‐directed task.] Developmental psychobiology. 2014 Sep;56(6):1416-22.</ref> <ref>Arnold AJ, Liddy JJ, Harris RC, Claxton LJ. T[https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/dev.21910 ask‐specific adaptations of postural sway in sitting infants.] Developmental Psychobiology. 2020 Jan;62(1):99-106.</ref>
Laura Claxton and colleagues<ref name=":10" /> <ref name=":11" /> studied the postural sway patterns of independently sitting infants while either holding or not holding a toy. Infants exhibited less postural sway when holding the toy. This reduction in sway allowed infants to look at and stabilise the toy in their hand.<ref name=":10">Claxton LJ, Strasser JM, Leung EJ, Ryu JH, O'Brien KM. [https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/24604626/ Sitting infants alter the magnitude and structure of postural sway when performing a manual goal‐directed task.] Developmental psychobiology. 2014 Sep;56(6):1416-22.</ref> <ref name=":11">Arnold AJ, Liddy JJ, Harris RC, Claxton LJ. T[https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/dev.21910 ask‐specific adaptations of postural sway in sitting infants.] Developmental Psychobiology. 2020 Jan;62(1):99-106.</ref>


== Sitting development is influenced by child rearing practices ==
== Sitting development is influenced by child rearing practices ==
Karasik and colleagues observed 5-month-olds and their mothers from six cultural groups around the world during one hour at home while they engaged in natural daily activities. They examined group differences in infants’ sitting proficiency, everyday opportunities to practice sitting, the surfaces on which sitting took place, and mothers’ proximity to sitting infants. Infants had opportunities to practice sitting in varied contexts—including ground, infant chairs, and raised surfaces.<ref name=":8">Karasik LB, Tamis-LeMonda CS, Adolph KE, Bornstein MH. [https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4767024/ Places and postures: A cross-cultural comparison of sitting in 5-month-olds]. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2015 Sep;46(8):1023-38.</ref>
Karasik and colleagues<ref name=":8" /> observed 5-month-olds and their mothers from six cultural groups around the world during one hour at home while they engaged in natural daily activities. They examined group differences in infants’ sitting proficiency, everyday opportunities to practise sitting, the surfaces on which sitting took place, and mothers’ proximity to sitting infants. Infants had opportunities to practise sitting in varied contexts—including on the ground, in infant chairs, and on raised surfaces.<ref name=":8">Karasik LB, Tamis-LeMonda CS, Adolph KE, Bornstein MH. [https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4767024/ Places and postures: A cross-cultural comparison of sitting in 5-month-olds]. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2015 Sep;46(8):1023-38.</ref>


Proficiency of sitting in these 5 month old infants varied considerably within and between cultural groups:   
Proficiency of sitting in these 5-month-old infants varied considerably within and between cultural groups:   


* 64% of the sample sat only with support from mother or furniture  
* 64% of the sample sat only with support from mother or furniture  
Line 129: Line 130:
These observations of infant sitting across cultures illustrate the how varied opportunities for practice and parental expectations influence the development of infant sitting.<ref name=":8" />  
These observations of infant sitting across cultures illustrate the how varied opportunities for practice and parental expectations influence the development of infant sitting.<ref name=":8" />  


Rachwani and colleagues describe the expereince of pre-sitting infants as follows:  
Rachwani and colleagues<ref name=":9" /> describe the experience of pre-sitting infants as follows:  


“Put another way, when pre-sitting infants practice sitting over and over on the floor, the couch, their caregiver’s knee, and so on, all the while engaged in various tasks of looking and interacting with objects and people, these experiences across a variety of surfaces and tasks, ensure the acquisition of behavioral flexibility.”<ref name=":9">Rachwani J, Soska KC, Adolph KE. [https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5690822/ Behavioral flexibility in learning to sit.] Developmental psychobiology. 2017 Dec;59(8):937-48.</ref>
“Put another way, when pre-sitting infants practice sitting over and over on the floor, the couch, their caregiver’s knee, and so on, all the while engaged in various tasks of looking and interacting with objects and people, these experiences across a variety of surfaces and tasks, ensure the acquisition of behavioral flexibility.”<ref name=":9">Rachwani J, Soska KC, Adolph KE. [https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5690822/ Behavioral flexibility in learning to sit.] Developmental psychobiology. 2017 Dec;59(8):937-48.</ref>
Line 135: Line 136:
Experience with varied body-environment relations may teach infants to find appropriate solutions for the current situation while also shoring up constituents common to all sitting postures.  
Experience with varied body-environment relations may teach infants to find appropriate solutions for the current situation while also shoring up constituents common to all sitting postures.  


For example, sitting on the floor and on the couch involve different demands on balance control. Shaking a rattle and turning to watch an event involve different constraints on balance. However, all of these situations strengthen infants’ torso and increase coordination to counteract destabilizing forces, thus allowing infants to adapt posture for a novel situation such as sitting on slopes.<ref name=":9" />
For example, sitting on the floor and on the couch involve different demands on balance control. Shaking a rattle and turning to watch an event involve different constraints on balance. However, all of these situations strengthen an infant’s torso and increase coordination to counteract destabilising forces, thus allowing the infant to adapt posture for a novel situation such as sitting on slopes.<ref name=":9" />


== Resources ==
== Resources ==

Revision as of 02:10, 20 September 2022

Original Editor - Pam Versfeld
Top Contributors - {{Special:Contributors/Template:Development of Sitting}}

Brief review of the biomechanics of sitting[edit | edit source]

Maintaining an upright sitting posture requires the ability to align the body segments so that the vertical projection of the centre of mass (COM), known as the line of gravity (LOG) falls within the base of support.

In infants, the COM when sitting lies within the chest.

When infants sit erect on a flat surface, their trunk is tilted forwards slightly so that the LOG runs just anterior to the flexion/extension axis of the hips.

This creates a flexion moment around the hips which is counteracted by the hip extensor muscles. Because the base of support (BOS) extends forwards over the lower limbs, the trunk can be tilted far forwards without the infant losing their balance.


However, in erect sitting, the LOG falls close to the posterior border of the BOS. This tilts the trunk backwards, moves the COP beyond the posterior edge of the BOS, which makes it difficult to maintain balance.

Infants learn over time to flex their neck and trunk to stop the LOG from moving past the posterior border of the BOS.

Pamv41.png

In sitting, the base of support in the side-to-side direction is relatively narrow. Any action that shifts weight onto one buttock moves the LOG to the edge of the BOS.

This is an unstable position, but stability can be restored by: realigning the position of the shoulder girdle relative to the pelvis; or by adapting a change-in-support strategy to increase the BOS in a sideways direction.[1]

Pamv49.png

Learning to sit has two components:[edit | edit source]

  1. Acquiring the strength and control needed to extend the spine and hold the head and trunk upright and steady.[2]
  2. Maintaining balance by adapting the alignment of the head and trunk relative to the BOS, and responding effectively to forces that perturb the head and trunk.[1]

Sitting does not just happen – it requires experience[edit | edit source]

The postural mechanisms that allow the infant to stabilise their head in space and keep their trunk erect and steady are initially learned from repeated experience of being carried upright and sitting with support over many months.

In the early months, carers provide maximum support for keeping the trunk erect and stable when carrying infants and also when holding them sitting on their laps.[3]

Sitting facing outwards on a carer’s lap allows the infant to visually explore the environment and attend to interesting things and events. This has an impact on postural set, which is influenced by arousal, motivation and curiosity.[4]

Pamv50.png

Over time, carers decrease the amount of support they provide and expect the infant to work harder to stay erect.[3] Infants also start to use their upper limbs to provide additional support by holding onto available surfaces.

Pamv52.png

Learning to extend the neck and spine[edit | edit source]

Over the first six months, repeated experience of being picked up, carried and supported in sitting provides the experience needed for strengthening the spinal muscles and learning to hold the head erect and extend the spine against gravity.

Infants first learn to keep the head upright and steady when support is provided at the level of the axilla. Over time, they progressively gain control over first the thoracic spine and then lumbar spine extension.[5][6][2]

This progressive development of extension of the spine can be seen when infants are supported in sitting at different levels: the axilla, thoracic spine and pelvis.

Pamv45.png

In typically developing infants, carers adapt the support they provide when lifting and carrying the infant, as well as when the infant sits on the carer’s lap.

As seen in these pictures, a small difference in the level of support can make a big difference to the infant's ability to sustain head and trunk extension.[5]

Pamv46.png

Postural orientation and balance[edit | edit source]

“Postural control is no longer considered simply a summation of static reflexes but, rather, a complex skill based on the interaction of dynamic sensorimotor processes [...] Sensory information from somatosensory, vestibular and visual systems is integrated, and the relative weights placed on each of these inputs depend on the goals of the movement task and the environmental context."[7]

Balance, also known as postural equilibrium, involves the coordination of movement strategies to stabilise the centre of body mass during both self-initiated and externally triggered disturbances of stability.

The specific response strategy selected depends not only on the characteristics of the external postural displacement, but also on the individual's expectations, goals and prior experience.[7]

Firstly anticipatory postural adjustments[edit | edit source]

Anticipatory postural adjustments (APA) occur prior to voluntary limb movement, and serve to maintain postural stability by initiating muscle contraction to compensate for destabilising forces associated with moving a limb.

An infant's experience with lifting, moving and vigorously shaking and banging rattles provide them with repeated opportunities to elicit APAs that stabilise the neck and trunk.

Pamv48.png

Another important postural strategy that emerges as infants gain more experience of sitting independently is the shift from a bottom up to a top down balance strategy whereby the infant learns to stabilise their head in space and adjust the alignment of their trunk segments to stay upright.

To do this, the alignment of the upper and lower thoracic segments and the pelvis are adjusted to keep the head upright and stable.

Pamv53.png

However, infants will still topple over sideways with the head and trunk moving as a unit when the lateral displacement of their COM is too far or too rapid.

Pamv60.png


With experience of independent sitting, infants also learn to use change-in-support strategies, involving grasping or stepping movements of the limbs to increase the BOS.[8]

When learning to balance in sitting, the most common change–in-support strategy used is reaching out to the side and taking weight on the hand when losing balance to the side. This action increases the BOS in a lateral direction, stops the falling COM and prevents a fall to the side.[9][8]

Importance of visual curiosity and desire to reach for toy[edit | edit source]

In very young infants, lifting the head and keeping it erect requires sustained effort, especially for an infant who has weakness of the neck and trunk extensors.

However, infants are usually willing to put in the effort needed into holding the head erect if there is something really interesting to look at such as a colorful object or an animated face that invites social interaction.

From about four months, typically developing infants take a great interest in reaching for, holding and manipulating toys. These actions require anticipatory and reactive muscle activity to stabilise the head and trunk and are important in providing infants with the experience needed for learning to sit independently with good balance.[10]

The natural progression of sitting development[edit | edit source]

The development of sitting involves increasing control over the neck and trunk segments that are involved in sitting erect and maintaining balance in the head, upper torso, lower torso and pelvis.

The development of sitting starts with the infant first learning to hold their head erect and steady when supported around the upper chest in an upright position.[6][2][11][12]

Pamv61.png

Over time and with practice, infants learn to stabilise their head and trunk when supported around the waist, and later with support around the pelvis.

At this stage, typically developing infants are often more interested in reaching for and grabbing an interesting toy than maintaining balance.[13]

Pmv64.png

This increasing level of control of the trunk on the pelvis is promoted by frequent opportunities to sit with support during every-day routines and handling.

Pamv65.png

Once the infant can stabilise their trunk on their pelvis, and sit briefly without arm or external support, typically developing infants start to pay attention to maintaining their balance as they reach out to connect with people and objects in their environment.[14]

Pamv66.png

With experience, infants learn to reach in all direction, pick up and move objects, and transition into and out of prone kneeling.

Pamv67.png

The role of postural sway[edit | edit source]

Humans and other species are unable to sit or stand perfectly still; their bodies continuously sway. Postural sway (or variability) inevitably emerges from the control of a system with many degrees of freedom.[15]

Postural sway has been traditionally viewed as a non-functional byproduct of a noisy motor system, leading to the concept that minimal sway was indicative of a healthy and stable postural system.

Contrary to these traditional views, Mark Carpenter and colleagues posit that postural sway may be used by the CNS as an exploratory mechanism to ensure that continuous dynamic inputs are provided by multiple sensory systems.[15]

Postural sway provides a continuous flow of sensory information from different sources (touch, proprioception, vestibular and visual) needed for estimating the position of the body as a whole in an ongoing manner.

Besides providing sensory feedback, these ongoing trunk movements in lateral and anterior-posterior directions allow the infant to explore the boundaries of the BOS and how far the COM can be moved without toppling over.

The extent of postural sway both in the anterior-posterior and side-to-side direction decreases with sitting experience. However, the complexity of movements of the COP increases.

Postural sway patterns are adapted when performing a concurrent goal directed task.[edit | edit source]

Laura Claxton and colleagues[16] [17] studied the postural sway patterns of independently sitting infants while either holding or not holding a toy. Infants exhibited less postural sway when holding the toy. This reduction in sway allowed infants to look at and stabilise the toy in their hand.[16] [17]

Sitting development is influenced by child rearing practices[edit | edit source]

Karasik and colleagues[18] observed 5-month-olds and their mothers from six cultural groups around the world during one hour at home while they engaged in natural daily activities. They examined group differences in infants’ sitting proficiency, everyday opportunities to practise sitting, the surfaces on which sitting took place, and mothers’ proximity to sitting infants. Infants had opportunities to practise sitting in varied contexts—including on the ground, in infant chairs, and on raised surfaces.[18]

Proficiency of sitting in these 5-month-old infants varied considerably within and between cultural groups:

  • 64% of the sample sat only with support from mother or furniture
  • 36% sat independently.

Some infants sat unsupported for 20+ minutes, in some cases so securely that mothers moved beyond arms’ reach of their infants even while infants sat on raised surfaces.

These observations of infant sitting across cultures illustrate the how varied opportunities for practice and parental expectations influence the development of infant sitting.[18]

Rachwani and colleagues[19] describe the experience of pre-sitting infants as follows:

“Put another way, when pre-sitting infants practice sitting over and over on the floor, the couch, their caregiver’s knee, and so on, all the while engaged in various tasks of looking and interacting with objects and people, these experiences across a variety of surfaces and tasks, ensure the acquisition of behavioral flexibility.”[19]

Experience with varied body-environment relations may teach infants to find appropriate solutions for the current situation while also shoring up constituents common to all sitting postures.

For example, sitting on the floor and on the couch involve different demands on balance control. Shaking a rattle and turning to watch an event involve different constraints on balance. However, all of these situations strengthen an infant’s torso and increase coordination to counteract destabilising forces, thus allowing the infant to adapt posture for a novel situation such as sitting on slopes.[19]

Resources[edit | edit source]

References[edit | edit source]

  1. 1.0 1.1 Kyvelidou A, Stuberg WA, Harbourne RT, Deffeyes JE, Blanke D, Stergiou N. Development of upper body coordination during sitting in typically developing infants. Pediatric research. 2009 May;65(5):553-8.
  2. 2.0 2.1 2.2 Pin TW, Butler PB, Cheung HM, Shum SL. Relationship between segmental trunk control and gross motor development in typically developing infants aged from 4 to 12 months: a pilot study. BMC pediatrics. 2019 Dec;19(1):1-9.
  3. 3.0 3.1 Duncan K, Goodworth A, Da Costa CS, Wininger M, Saavedra S. Parent handling of typical infants varies segmentally across development of postural control. Experimental brain research. 2018 Mar;236(3):645-54.
  4. Franchak JM. Changing opportunities for learning in everyday life: Infant body position over the first year. Infancy. 2019 Mar;24(2):187-209.
  5. 5.0 5.1 Saavedra SL, van Donkelaar P, Woollacott MH. Learning about gravity: segmental assessment of upright control as infants develop independent sitting. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2012 Oct 15;108(8):2215-29.
  6. 6.0 6.1 Sangkarit N, Siritaratiwat W, Bennett S, Tapanya W. Factors Associating with the Segmental Postural Control during Sitting in Moderate-to-Late Preterm Infants via Longitudinal Study. Children. 2021 Sep 26;8(10):851.
  7. 7.0 7.1 Horak FB. Postural orientation and equilibrium: what do we need to know about neural control of balance to prevent falls?. Age and ageing. 2006 Sep 1;35(suppl_2):ii7-11.
  8. 8.0 8.1 Maki BE, McIlroy WE. The role of limb movements in maintaining upright stance: the “change-in-support” strategy. Physical therapy. 1997 May 1;77(5):488-507.
  9. Kyvelidou A, Stuberg WA, Harbourne RT, Deffeyes JE, Blanke D, Stergiou N. Development of upper body coordination during sitting in typically developing infants. Pediatric research. 2009 May;65(5):553-8.
  10. Bertenthal B, Von Hofsten C. Eye, head and trunk control: The foundation for manual development. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 1998 Mar 4;22(4):515-20.
  11. van Iersel PA, la Bastide-van Gemert S, Wu YC, Hadders-Algra M. Alberta Infant Motor Scale: Cross-cultural analysis of gross motor development in Dutch and Canadian infants and introduction of Dutch norms. Early Human Development. 2020 Dec 1;151:105239.
  12. Gontijo AP, de Melo Mambrini JV, Mancini MC. Cross-country validity of the Alberta Infant Motor Scale using a Brazilian sample. Brazilian Journal of Physical Therapy. 2021 Jul 1;25(4):444-9.
  13. Mlincek MM, Roemer EJ, Kraemer C, Iverson JM. Posture Matters: Object Manipulation During the Transition to Arms-Free Sitting in Infants at Elevated vs. Typical Likelihood for Autism Spectrum Disorder. Physical & Occupational Therapy In Pediatrics. 2022 Jan 9:1-5.
  14. Harbourne RT, Lobo MA, Karst GM, Galloway JC. Sit happens: Does sitting development perturb reaching development, or vice versa?. Infant Behavior and Development. 2013 Jun 1;36(3):438-50.
  15. 15.0 15.1 Carpenter MG, Murnaghan CD, Inglis JT. Shifting the balance: evidence of an exploratory role for postural sway. Neuroscience. 2010 Nov 24;171(1):196-204.
  16. 16.0 16.1 Claxton LJ, Strasser JM, Leung EJ, Ryu JH, O'Brien KM. Sitting infants alter the magnitude and structure of postural sway when performing a manual goal‐directed task. Developmental psychobiology. 2014 Sep;56(6):1416-22.
  17. 17.0 17.1 Arnold AJ, Liddy JJ, Harris RC, Claxton LJ. Task‐specific adaptations of postural sway in sitting infants. Developmental Psychobiology. 2020 Jan;62(1):99-106.
  18. 18.0 18.1 18.2 Karasik LB, Tamis-LeMonda CS, Adolph KE, Bornstein MH. Places and postures: A cross-cultural comparison of sitting in 5-month-olds. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2015 Sep;46(8):1023-38.
  19. 19.0 19.1 19.2 Rachwani J, Soska KC, Adolph KE. Behavioral flexibility in learning to sit. Developmental psychobiology. 2017 Dec;59(8):937-48.