Hand Function 7-24 Month Period: Difference between revisions

No edit summary
No edit summary
Line 40: Line 40:


== Grasp Patterns ==
== Grasp Patterns ==
As the infant learns to sit independently, they have more efficient reaching as their arms are freed up from being used as a support.  The grasps transition from a reflexive palmar grasp to a radial palmar grasp to a pincer grasp.  These fine motor changes allow for more detailed manipulation and play of objects.  Releasing objects voluntarily begins around 7-8 months initially awkwardly moving to more purposeful.  Around 12 months, infants use fingering and hand-hand manipulations to investigate objects.<ref name=":0">Gee BM, Engle J, Parker C, Stanger M, Call H, Peterson TW. [https://journals.healio.com/doi/pdf/10.3928/24761222-20190813-03 Frequency and duration of developmental fine motor patterns in infants and toddlers: a pilot cohort study]. Annals of International Occupational Therapy. 2020 Jan 1;3(1):21-8.</ref>  
When infants learn to sit independently they become more efficient at reaching and grasping as their arms are no longer needed for support.  Their grasp transitions from a reflexive palmar grasp to a radial palmar grasp to a pincer grasp.  These fine motor adaptations facilitate more detailed manipulation and play of objects.  Using new grasps, infants begin releasing objects awkwardly around 7-8 months moving to more purposeful movements as they age.  Around 12 months old, infants use fingering and hand-hand manipulations to investigate objects.<ref name=":0">Gee BM, Engle J, Parker C, Stanger M, Call H, Peterson TW. [https://journals.healio.com/doi/pdf/10.3928/24761222-20190813-03 Frequency and duration of developmental fine motor patterns in infants and toddlers: a pilot cohort study]. Annals of International Occupational Therapy. 2020 Jan 1;3(1):21-8.</ref>  Repetitive trials allows them to find a finger pattern that is best suited to the size and shape of different objects.
 
As the infants learn to manipulate objects with the above hand-object actions, they also learn which grasp is best suited for each object. Repetitive trials allows them to find a finger pattern that is best suited to the size and shape of different objects.


Grasp patterns commonly seen during this period:
Grasp patterns commonly seen during this period:
Line 51: Line 49:
* Circular grasp: used to grasp a ball or flat round object
* Circular grasp: used to grasp a ball or flat round object
* Chuck grasp: using the thumb, forefinger and ring finger; complex grasp typically for 4-5 years old  
* Chuck grasp: using the thumb, forefinger and ring finger; complex grasp typically for 4-5 years old  
* Key grasp: used to grasp small flat surface between thumb and side of forefinger, other fingers flexed to provide support<ref>[https://skillsforaction.com/different-ways-we-use-our-hands-everyday-function Skills for Action.  Accessible at: https://skillsforaction.com/different-ways-we-use-our-hands-everyday-function]</ref>  
* Key grasp: used to grasp small flat surface between thumb and side of forefinger, other fingers flexed to provide support<ref>[https://skillsforaction.com/different-ways-we-use-our-hands-everyday-function Skills for Action.  Accessible at: https://skillsforaction.com/different-ways-we-use-our-hands-everyday-function]</ref><ref name=":2" />


=== Ball Activities ===
== Small Objects ==
[[File:Babyball(1).jpg|thumb]]
Around 10-11 months old, infants begin to poke and pick up small objects. Poking entails using an extended forefinger while picking involves bending and extending the fingers using a raking action. As time progresses, infants will transition to a pincer or key grip to pick up small objects.<ref name=":2" />
The use of a ball play in infancy creates a variety of experience about how balls function. Infants learn to visually track moving balls as well as predict where the ball will stop. Anticipating the future location of a moving ball is an important skill for learning to catch a ball. Ball play with partners allows the infants to discover different ways balls move, how to catch and throw and to observe their partners actions.


=== Small Objects ===
== Ball Activities ==
Around 10-11 months, infants begin to poke and pick up small objects. Poking entails using an extended foreginger while picking up involves bending and extending the fingers using a raking action. As time progresses, infants will transition to a pincer or key grip to pick up small objects.
[[File:Babyball(1).jpg|alt=|right|frameless|300x300px]]
Ball play in infancy creates a variety of experiences about how balls function. Infants learn to visually track moving balls as well as predict where the ball will stop moving. Anticipating the future location of a moving ball is an important skill for learning to catch a ball. Ball play with partners allows the infants to observe their partners actions, discover different ways balls move,  and how to catch and throw.<ref name=":2" />


=== Social interaction and communication gestures ===
== Social interaction and communication gestures ==
During the 7-12 month period, infants use their hands to communicate and for social interaction. Some of the various gestures you might see during this time are listed below:
During the 7-12 month period, infants use their hands for social interaction and communication. Some of the various gestures you might see during this stage are listed below:


* waving
* waving
Line 68: Line 66:
* raising hands for hooray
* raising hands for hooray
* high five
* high five
* copying actions
* copying actions<ref name=":2" />


== 12-24 Months ==
== 12-24 Months ==

Revision as of 20:08, 3 June 2022

Intro[edit | edit source]

Infants transitioning into toddlerhood will demonstrate more complex use of their hands. In early infancy, hand use was subjected to random spontaneous movements of the arms. As they age, infants engage in intentional movement patterns to reach and grasp object they seek.[1] Successful reaching entails locating the target while opening and aligning the hand in preparation to grasp the object. Developmentally, reaching precedes grasping.[2] Between 9-19 months old, infants can plan ahead before reaching a target.[3] During this time period, reaching and grasping is not limited to visual targets, as infants can reach auditory targets as well.[2] Throughout the 7-24 month period, infants will use their hands in a variety of ways along with a variety of finger grasps to manipulate, learn and play with objects.[1]

Development of Hand Function[edit | edit source]

Development of hand function for infants occurs in two processes; cascades in development and perception-action loops with embedded exploration and selection.[1]

  • Cascades in Development:
    • theory based on learned new skills are a combination of established development with new skill acquisition
  • Perception-Action Loops with Embedded Exploration and Selection:
    • infants will use repeated trials to learn new skills
    • with each opportunity they learn how to vary their movement to attain success
      • provides different sensory feedback as a consequence of their movement[1]

Perception[edit | edit source]

The way in which we gather information about the events within the environment (the physical structure and the events occuring) is referred to as perception. We attain this information as an active brain process in recognising patterns from the steady flow of sensory information. Examples of perceiving sensory information is through tasting, feeling, looking and listening. For infants, hand use provides opportunities to learn about the properties of objects through their feeling sensation.[1]

7-12 Month Period[edit | edit source]

The hands serve several purposes during the 7-12 month period. Their trifold functions are listed below:

using hand for support
  1. As part of the postural and locomotor systems
    • learning to balance in sitting, crawling and buttock shuffling, cruising
  2. For communication and in social interactions
  3. For picking up, moving and manipulating objects
    • to explore their different properties and what can be done with them[1]

Hand-Object Actions[edit | edit source]

During the 7-12 month time period, infants will use their hands to interact with objects with varied complex actions. These hand-object behaviours are listed below:

  • shaking
  • banging
  • patting, poking, stroking, flapping with the fingers
  • turning objects over
  • passing from one hand to the other
  • throwing and dropping objects
  • picking up, moving and dropping small and large objects[1]

** By 12 months, infants reach and grasp motions are similar to that of an adult[4]

Grasp Patterns[edit | edit source]

When infants learn to sit independently they become more efficient at reaching and grasping as their arms are no longer needed for support. Their grasp transitions from a reflexive palmar grasp to a radial palmar grasp to a pincer grasp. These fine motor adaptations facilitate more detailed manipulation and play of objects. Using new grasps, infants begin releasing objects awkwardly around 7-8 months moving to more purposeful movements as they age. Around 12 months old, infants use fingering and hand-hand manipulations to investigate objects.[5] Repetitive trials allows them to find a finger pattern that is best suited to the size and shape of different objects.

Grasp patterns commonly seen during this period:

  • Hook grasp: used to grasp small objects; fingers flexed at all joints
  • Power grasp: used to hold a cylindrical object between fingers and thumb
  • Palmar grasp: used to hold objects or tools with a long handle
  • Circular grasp: used to grasp a ball or flat round object
  • Chuck grasp: using the thumb, forefinger and ring finger; complex grasp typically for 4-5 years old
  • Key grasp: used to grasp small flat surface between thumb and side of forefinger, other fingers flexed to provide support[6][1]

Small Objects[edit | edit source]

Around 10-11 months old, infants begin to poke and pick up small objects. Poking entails using an extended forefinger while picking involves bending and extending the fingers using a raking action. As time progresses, infants will transition to a pincer or key grip to pick up small objects.[1]

Ball Activities[edit | edit source]

Ball play in infancy creates a variety of experiences about how balls function. Infants learn to visually track moving balls as well as predict where the ball will stop moving. Anticipating the future location of a moving ball is an important skill for learning to catch a ball. Ball play with partners allows the infants to observe their partners actions, discover different ways balls move, and how to catch and throw.[1]

Social interaction and communication gestures[edit | edit source]

During the 7-12 month period, infants use their hands for social interaction and communication. Some of the various gestures you might see during this stage are listed below:

  • waving
  • arm pointing
  • clapping hands
  • raising hands for hooray
  • high five
  • copying actions[1]

12-24 Months[edit | edit source]

At approximately 15 to 18 months, toddlers have improved spatial relations and visual perceptual skills that contribute to an improved ability to manipulate objects in hand (e.g., single-handed management of food, toys). This improvement contributes to the development of bilateral hand use (e.g., self-feeding, toys, simple clothing[5]

Task Structure[edit | edit source]

During the 12-24 month stage, toddlers use their hands in a more goal-directed action. They will explore objects manipulating and moving it comparing it other objects. Goal-oriented tasks requires an understanding of the series of actions needed to complete the task otherwise known as task structure. Three main components needed to complete a task structure are listed below:

  1. visual information gathered for planning movements
  2. predict and adapt posture in response to limb movements
  3. complete a sequence of movements

Toddlers learn to perform tasks with greater accuracy over time and through repeated experiences. During play activities, toddlers will adapt their grip, their body position and the pulling force applied through their arms to accommodate for the specific task. They quickly learn how to plan, execute and adapt their movements to attain their goal.

Posting Activities[edit | edit source]

Posting or inserting refers to an activity where the toddler places objects through small openings. Often times, posting activities include small balls, rods or flat disks inserted into holes in the lids of a container. Through the undertaking, toddlers learn the size and shape of objects that can fit through the various holes.

Duration of Play[edit | edit source]

Toddlers age 12-24 months, have brief sustained periods of focused attention to a task before moving onto another activity. These quick bursts of focused play are not a sign of poor attention span but one of using the most effective way to learn about properties and affordances of many different objects. A study by Herzberg et al.2022 showed that toddlers object interactions were on (average 9.8 seconds) and varied, with transitions among dozens of toys and non-tpys.[7]

Resources[edit | edit source]

https://babynavigator.com/resources/

https://babynavigator.com/wp-content/uploads/2021/04/Milestones-that-Matter-Mo

References[edit | edit source]

  1. 1.00 1.01 1.02 1.03 1.04 1.05 1.06 1.07 1.08 1.09 1.10 Versfeld, P. Hand Function 7-24 Months Course. Physioplus. 2022
  2. 2.0 2.1 Chinn LK, Noonan CF, Hoffmann M, Lockman JJ. Development of infant reaching strategies to tactile targets on the face. Frontiers in psychology. 2019:9.
  3. Kaur M, Detherage A, Needham AW. Unconventional tool use in infants: Using a familiar tool in a novel way in the second year of life. Cognitive Development. 2020 Apr 1;54:100881.
  4. Sacrey LA, Zwaigenbaum L, Bryson S, Brian J, Smith IM. The reach-to-grasp movement in infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder: a high-risk sibling cohort study. Journal of neurodevelopmental disorders. 2018 Dec;10(1):1-1.
  5. 5.0 5.1 Gee BM, Engle J, Parker C, Stanger M, Call H, Peterson TW. Frequency and duration of developmental fine motor patterns in infants and toddlers: a pilot cohort study. Annals of International Occupational Therapy. 2020 Jan 1;3(1):21-8.
  6. Skills for Action. Accessible at: https://skillsforaction.com/different-ways-we-use-our-hands-everyday-function
  7. Herzberg O, Fletcher KK, Schatz JL, Adolph KE, Tamis‐LeMonda CS. Infant exuberant object play at home: Immense amounts of time‐distributed, variable practice. Child development. 2022 Jan;93(1):150-64.