Hand Function 7-24 Month Period: Difference between revisions

No edit summary
No edit summary
Line 3: Line 3:


== Intro ==
== Intro ==
Infants transitioning into toddlerhood will demonstrate increasingly good use of their hands.  In early infancy, spontaneous movements help the toddler make use of their hands.  As they age, they move into intentional movement patterns to reach and grasp the object they seek.<ref>Versfeld, P.  Hand Function 7-24 Months Course. Physioplus. 2022</ref>  Successful reaching entails locating the target while opening and aligning the hand in preparation to grasp the object.  Developmentally, reaching precedes grasping.<ref name=":1">Chinn LK, Noonan CF, Hoffmann M, Lockman JJ. [https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00009/full Development of infant reaching strategies to tactile targets on the face]. Frontiers in psychology. 2019:9.</ref>  Between 9-19 months old, infants are able to plan ahead regarding the action of reaching a target.<ref>Kaur M, Detherage A, Needham AW. [https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/S0885201420300356 Unconventional tool use in infants: Using a familiar tool in a novel way in the second year of life]. Cognitive Development. 2020 Apr 1;54:100881.</ref>  Reaching and grasping is not limited to visual targets, as infants can reach auditory targets as well.<ref name=":1" /> Throughout the 7-24 month period, infants will use their hands in a variety of ways along with a variety of finger grasps to manipulate, learn and play with objects
Infants transitioning into toddlerhood will demonstrate increasingly good use of their hands.  In early infancy, spontaneous movements help the toddler make use of their hands.  As they age, they move into intentional movement patterns to reach and grasp the object they seek.<ref>Versfeld, P.  Hand Function 7-24 Months Course. Physioplus. 2022</ref>  Successful reaching entails locating the target while opening and aligning the hand in preparation to grasp the object.  Developmentally, reaching precedes grasping.<ref name=":1">Chinn LK, Noonan CF, Hoffmann M, Lockman JJ. [https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00009/full Development of infant reaching strategies to tactile targets on the face]. Frontiers in psychology. 2019:9.</ref>  Between 9-19 months old, infants are able to plan ahead regarding the action of reaching a target.<ref>Kaur M, Detherage A, Needham AW. [https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/S0885201420300356 Unconventional tool use in infants: Using a familiar tool in a novel way in the second year of life]. Cognitive Development. 2020 Apr 1;54:100881.</ref>  Reaching and grasping is not limited to visual targets, as infants can reach auditory targets as well.<ref name=":1" /> Throughout the 7-24 month period, infants will use their hands in a variety of ways along with a variety of finger grasps to manipulate, learn and play with objects.


== Development of Hand Function ==
== Development of Hand Function ==

Revision as of 13:00, 3 June 2022

Intro[edit | edit source]

Infants transitioning into toddlerhood will demonstrate increasingly good use of their hands. In early infancy, spontaneous movements help the toddler make use of their hands. As they age, they move into intentional movement patterns to reach and grasp the object they seek.[1] Successful reaching entails locating the target while opening and aligning the hand in preparation to grasp the object. Developmentally, reaching precedes grasping.[2] Between 9-19 months old, infants are able to plan ahead regarding the action of reaching a target.[3] Reaching and grasping is not limited to visual targets, as infants can reach auditory targets as well.[2] Throughout the 7-24 month period, infants will use their hands in a variety of ways along with a variety of finger grasps to manipulate, learn and play with objects.

Development of Hand Function[edit | edit source]

Development of hand function for infants occurs in two processes; cascades in development and perception-action loops with embedded exploration and selection.

Cascades in Development[edit | edit source]

Hand development in infancy is formed by previous learned skills. New skills are a combination of established development with new skill acquistion. Hand function begins by spontaneous movement transitioning into intentional goal-directed play.

Perception-Action Loops with Embedded Exploration and Selection[edit | edit source]

Infants will use repeated trials to learn new skills. With each opportunity they learn how to vary their movement to attain success. Some patterns of movements will be fruitful than others. Performing an action repetitively allows for different sensory feedback as a consequence of their movement. This process of executing various movement patterns over repetitive trials refers to the perception-action loop.

Perception[edit | edit source]

The way in which we gather information about the events within the environment (the physical structure and the events occuring) is referred to as perception. We attain this information through an active process of the brain recognising patterns from the steady flow of sensory information. Some of the ways in which we perceive sensory information is through tasting, feeling, looking and listening. The use of hands provides the sensory function of feeling to learn about properties of the object.

7-12 Month Period[edit | edit source]

The purposes of the hand during this period is trifold:

  1. As part of the postural and locomotor systems
    • learning to balance in sitting, crawling and buttock shuffling, cruising
  2. For communication and in social interactions
  3. For picking up, moving and manipulating objects
    • to explore their different properties and what can be done with them

Hand-Object Actions[edit | edit source]

During the 7-12 month time period, infants will use their hands to interact with objects with varied complex actions:

  • shaking
  • banging
  • patting, poking, stroking, flapping with the fingers
  • turning objects over
  • passing from one hand to the other
  • throwing and dropping objects
  • picking up, moving and dropping small and large objects

** By 12 months, infants reach and grasping motion are similar to that of an adult[4]

Grasp Patterns[edit | edit source]

As the infant learns to sit independently, they have more efficient reaching as their arms are freed up from being used as a support. The grasps transition from a reflexive palmar grasp to a radial palmar grasp to a pincer grasp. These fine motor changes allow for more detailed manipulation and play of objects. Releasing objects voluntarily begins around 7-8 months initially awkwardly moving to more purposeful. Around 12 months, infants use fingering and hand-hand manipulations to investigate objects.[5]

As the infants learn to manipulate objects with the above hand-object actions, they also learn which grasp is best suited for each object. Repetitive trials allows them to find a finger pattern that is best suited to the size and shape of different objects.

Grasp patterns commonly seen during this period:

  • Hook grasp: used to grasp small objects; fingers flexed at all joints
  • Power grasp: used to hold a cylindrical object between fingers and thumb
  • Palmar grasp: used to hold objects or tools with a long handle
  • Circular grasp: used to grasp a ball or flat round object
  • Chuck grasp: using the thumb, forefinger and ring finger; complex grasp typically for 4-5 years old
  • Key grasp: used to grasp small flat surface between thumb and side of forefinger, other fingers flexed to provide support[6]

Ball Activities[edit | edit source]

Babyball(1).jpg

The use of a ball play in infancy creates a variety of experience about how balls function. Infants learn to visually track moving balls as well as predict where the ball will stop. Anticipating the future location of a moving ball is an important skill for learning to catch a ball. Ball play with partners allows the infants to discover different ways balls move, how to catch and throw and to observe their partners actions.

Small Objects[edit | edit source]

Around 10-11 months, infants begin to poke and pick up small objects. Poking entails using an extended foreginger while picking up involves bending and extending the fingers using a raking action. As time progresses, infants will transition to a pincer or key grip to pick up small objects.

Social interaction and communication gestures[edit | edit source]

During the 7-12 month period, infants use their hands to communicate and for social interaction. Some of the various gestures you might see during this time are listed below:

  • waving
  • arm pointing
  • clapping hands
  • raising hands for hooray
  • high five
  • copying actions

12-24 Months[edit | edit source]

At approximately 15 to 18 months, toddlers have improved spatial relations and visual perceptual skills that contribute to an improved ability to manipulate objects in hand (e.g., single-handed management of food, toys). This improvement contributes to the development of bilateral hand use (e.g., self-feeding, toys, simple clothing[5]

Task Structure[edit | edit source]

During the 12-24 month stage, toddlers use their hands in a more goal-directed action. They will explore objects manipulating and moving it comparing it other objects. Goal-oriented tasks requires an understanding of the series of actions needed to complete the task otherwise known as task structure. Three main components needed to complete a task structure are listed below:

  1. visual information gathered for planning movements
  2. predict and adapt posture in response to limb movements
  3. complete a sequence of movements

Toddlers learn to perform tasks with greater accuracy over time and through repeated experiences. During play activities, toddlers will adapt their grip, their body position and the pulling force applied through their arms to accommodate for the specific task. They quickly learn how to plan, execute and adapt their movements to attain their goal.

Posting Activities[edit | edit source]

Posting or inserting refers to an activity where the toddler places objects through small openings. Often times, posting activities include small balls, rods or flat disks inserted into holes in the lids of a container. Through the undertaking, toddlers learn the size and shape of objects that can fit through the various holes.

Duration of Play[edit | edit source]

Toddlers age 12-24 months, have brief sustained periods of focused attention to a task before moving onto another activity. These quick bursts of focused play are not a sign of poor attention span but one of using the most effective way to learn about properties and affordances of many different objects. A study by Herzberg et al.2022 showed that toddlers object interactions were on (average 9.8 seconds) and varied, with transitions among dozens of toys and non-tpys.[7]

Resources[edit | edit source]

https://babynavigator.com/resources/

https://babynavigator.com/wp-content/uploads/2021/04/Milestones-that-Matter-Mo

References[edit | edit source]

  1. Versfeld, P. Hand Function 7-24 Months Course. Physioplus. 2022
  2. 2.0 2.1 Chinn LK, Noonan CF, Hoffmann M, Lockman JJ. Development of infant reaching strategies to tactile targets on the face. Frontiers in psychology. 2019:9.
  3. Kaur M, Detherage A, Needham AW. Unconventional tool use in infants: Using a familiar tool in a novel way in the second year of life. Cognitive Development. 2020 Apr 1;54:100881.
  4. Sacrey LA, Zwaigenbaum L, Bryson S, Brian J, Smith IM. The reach-to-grasp movement in infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder: a high-risk sibling cohort study. Journal of neurodevelopmental disorders. 2018 Dec;10(1):1-1.
  5. 5.0 5.1 Gee BM, Engle J, Parker C, Stanger M, Call H, Peterson TW. Frequency and duration of developmental fine motor patterns in infants and toddlers: a pilot cohort study. Annals of International Occupational Therapy. 2020 Jan 1;3(1):21-8.
  6. Skills for Action. Accessible at: https://skillsforaction.com/different-ways-we-use-our-hands-everyday-function
  7. Herzberg O, Fletcher KK, Schatz JL, Adolph KE, Tamis‐LeMonda CS. Infant exuberant object play at home: Immense amounts of time‐distributed, variable practice. Child development. 2022 Jan;93(1):150-64.